@article{Hommers1991, author = {Hommers, Wilfried}, title = {Ist das Konzept des Anti-Typus entbehrlich? Eine Antwort des Mischverteilungsansatzes zu einem IPC-Anti-Typ : (Prof. Dr. Dr. G.A. Lienert zum 70. Geburtstag)}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-44109}, year = {1991}, abstract = {Durch multidimensional skalierte Strukturen von Pers{\"o}nlichkeitsprofilen wurde eine AlternativHypothese f{\"u}r konfigurationsfrequenzanalytisch erlangte Konfigurationen begr{\"u}ndet. Diese Alternativ- Hypothese besagte, daß die Anti-Typus-Konfigurationen eines dreivariaten 2x2x2- Datenvektors, aber auch die Neutral-Konfigurationen lediglich Folgen der Mischverteilung der Antworten zweier latenter, den Typen-Konfigurationen zuzuordnenden Klassen darstellen. Diese Hypothese wird an einem Datensatz zur Verteilung dichotomisierter Kontroll{\"u}berzeugungen gepr{\"u}ft und best{\"a}tigt. Das dabei konzipierte Konfigurale Fehlermodell (KFM) wird generell zur Erg{\"a}nzung von Konfigurationsfrequenzanalysen (KFA) vorgeschlagen.}, subject = {Klinische Psychologie / Zeitschrift /Psychiatrie / Zeitschrift / Klinische Psychiatrie / Psychotherapie}, language = {de} } @incollection{Hommers1991, author = {Hommers, Wilfried}, title = {Zum Gegenstand der Rechtspsychologie}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-87014}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}t W{\"u}rzburg}, year = {1991}, abstract = {No abstract available.}, subject = {Rechtspsychologie}, language = {de} } @incollection{Ellgring1991, author = {Ellgring, Johann Heinrich}, title = {Introduction. The use of Video for behavior description and intervention}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-42019}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}t W{\"u}rzburg}, year = {1991}, abstract = {No abstract available}, language = {en} } @article{WeinertSchneiderAsendorpfetal.1991, author = {Weinert, Franz E. and Schneider, Wolfgang and Asendorpf, Jens B. and Bullock, M. and Helmke, Andreas and Knopf, Monika and Nunner-Winkler, G. and Stern, E. and Strube, G. and Weber, Andreas}, title = {Intra- und interindividuelle Unterschiede in der psychischen Entwicklung von Kindern}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-86627}, year = {1991}, abstract = {No abstract available.}, subject = {Kind}, language = {de} } @article{KurtzSchneiderCarretal.1990, author = {Kurtz, Beth E. and Schneider, Wolfgang and Carr, Martha and Borkowski, John G. and Rellinger, Elizabeth}, title = {Strategy instruction and attributional beliefs in West Germany and the United States: Do teachers foster metacognitive development?}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62145}, year = {1990}, abstract = {Previous research has shown German children to be more strategic on sort-recall memory tasks than their American age-mates, and to show fewer effort-related attributions. We conducted this study to determine if those differences are due to systematic differences in the strategy instruction and attributional beliefs of German and U.S. teachers, and to explore metacognitive instructional practices in the two countries. Teachers responded to a questionnaire that inquired about their use of strategy instructions, fostering of reflective thinking in pupils, sources of children's learning problems, and modeling of metacognitive skills such as monitoring. The second part of the questionnaire asked about the reasons underlying children's academic successes and failures. German teachers reported more instruction of task-specific strategies, while American teachers showed more effort-related attributions. The types of strategies instructed and types of learning problems most frequently described varied across the two countries, and also according to how many years the teachers had taught. Results were discussed regarding their implications for metacognitive developmental theory, particularly regarding culture and other environmental influences on the development of controlled processing.}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @article{PressleyBorkowskiSchneider1990, author = {Pressley, Michael and Borkowski, John G. and Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {Good information processing: What it is and how education can promote it}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62127}, year = {1990}, abstract = {The nature of good information processing is outlined as determined by intact neurology, information stored in long-term memory, and general cognitive tendencies, attitudes, and styles. Educators can promote the development of good information processing by promoting what is in long-term memory. This can be accomplished by teaching important literary, scientific, and cultural knowledge; teaching strategies; motivating the acquisition and use of important conceptual knowledge and strategies; and encouraging the general tendencies supporting good information processing. Good information processing can be produced by years of appropriate educational input. Good information processors cannot be produced by short-term interventions.}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @article{SodianSchneider1990, author = {Sodian, Beate and Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {Children's understanding of cognitive cueing: How to manipulate cues to fool a competitor}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62132}, year = {1990}, abstract = {4-6-year-old children's understanding of cognitive cuing was studied in 2 experiments using a strategic interaction paradigm. Ghildren could fool a competitor by hiding targets in locations that were labeled with semantically weakly associated cues and help a cooperative partner by hiding them in semantically highly associated locations. Very few 4-year-olds, half the 5-year-olds, and almost all 6-year-olds appropriately chose semantically highly vs. weakly associated hiding places to make the targets easy vs. difficult to find. The second experiment showed that 4-year-olds did not strategically manipulate cues as sources of information, although they themselves proficiently used them as such in a search task. These findings are discussed with regard to research on children's developing understanding of origins of knowledge and belief and with regard to recent claims that young preschoolers possess a metacognitive understanding of cognitive cuing.}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @incollection{Ellgring1990, author = {Ellgring, Johann Heinrich}, title = {Sozialpsychologie: {\"A}tiologie/Bedingungsanalyse}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-41991}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}t W{\"u}rzburg}, year = {1990}, abstract = {No abstract available}, language = {de} } @article{Hommers1990, author = {Hommers, Wilfried}, title = {Entschuldigung, Drittentsch{\"a}digung und Schadensh{\"o}he als Einflußgr{\"o}ßen des Urteils von Kindern und Erwachsenen}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-44095}, year = {1990}, abstract = {Kinder aus 3 Altersgruppen und Erwachsene beurteilten die verdiente Strafe f{\"u}r in Geschichten dargestellte Sachsch{\"a}den, die aus Versehen oder aus Wut erfolgten. In den Geschichten wurde die Schadensh{\"o}he variiert und außerdem, ob sich der T{\"a}ter entschuldigte oder nicht bzw. ob der Gesch{\"a}digte von einem Dritten eine Entsch{\"a}digung erhielt oder nicht. Entschuldigung und Schadensh{\"o}he hatten altersstabile Wirkungen, Entschuldigung etwa zweimal so stark wie die Schadensh{\"o}he. Entschuldigung und Drittentsch{\"a}digung hatten etwa gleichgroße Wirkungen, außer bei den Erwachsenen, bei denen die Wirkung der Drittentsch{\"a}digung etwa auf die St{\"a}rke der Schadenswirkung abnahm. Der urspr{\"u}ngliche Schaden war auch bei erfolgter Entsch{\"a}digung, wenngleich schw{\"a}cher, wirksam. Die Wirkungen der Ersatzleistung durch den T{\"a}ter auf Strafurteile waren jedoch nicht v{\"o}llig durch die Wirkungen von Entschuldigung und Entsch{\"a}digung, erkl{\"a}rbar.}, subject = {Angewandte Psychologie / Zeitschrift / Experimentelle Psychologie}, language = {de} } @article{SchneiderUhl1990, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang and Uhl, Christhild}, title = {Metaged{\"a}chtnis, Strategienutzung und Ged{\"a}chtnisleistung: Vergleichende Analysen bei Kindern, j{\"u}ngeren Erwachsenen und alten Menschen}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-69418}, year = {1990}, abstract = {Am Beispiel einer semantischen Kategorisierungsaufgabe (sort-recall task) wurde der Frage nachgegangen, in welchen Bestimmungsgr{\"o}ßen sich die Ged{\"a}chtnisleistungen von Schulkindern, j{\"u}ngeren sowie {\"a}lteren Erwachsenen voneinander unterscheiden. Es wurde angenommen, daß f{\"u}r diese drei Altersgruppen Ged{\"a}chtnisleistungen bei dieser Aufgabe in unterschiedlicher Weise durch Strategie- und Wissensaspekte bestimmt sind. Die im Vergleich zu Schulkindern und {\"a}lteren Erwachsenen {\"u}blicherweise besseren Leistungen j{\"u}ngerer Erwachsener sollten demnach im wesentlichen auf die konsequentere Nutzung von Ged{\"a}chtnisstrategien r{\"u}ckf{\"u}hrbar sein. Erwartet wurde weiterhin, daß die bei Schulkindern und {\"a}lteren Erwachsenen oft vorfindbaren "Produktionsdefizite" in der Strategienutzung unterschiedliche Ursachen haben : fehlt es bei den Sch{\"u}lern am notwendigen Ged{\"a}chtniswissen (Metaged{\"a}chtnis), so sind die Defizite der {\"a}lteren Menschen vorwiegend auf die mangelnde Erfahrung mit der Aufgabe zur{\"u}ckzuf{\"u}hren. Diese Annahmen wurden in einer Studie mit je 24 Probanden aus den drei genannten Altersgruppen {\"u}berpr{\"u}ft. W{\"a}hrend sich das erwartete Produktionsdefizit bei den Kindern auf unzureichendes Metaged{\"a}chtnis zur{\"u}ckf{\"u}hren ließ, gab es wenig Anhaltspunkte daf{\"u}r, daß das Strategiedefizit {\"a}lterer Menschen in wesentlichen auf mangelnde Aufgabenerfahrung r{\"u}ckf{\"u}hrbar ist. Leistungsunterschiede zwischen j{\"u}ngeren und {\"a}lteren Erwachsenen beruhen nicht auf unterschiedlichem Ged{\"a}chtniswissen, sondern d{\"u}rften auf dem kombinierten Einfluß von Strategie- und Kapazit{\"a}tsdefiziten basieren.}, subject = {Metaged{\"a}chtnis}, language = {de} } @article{Ellgring1989, author = {Ellgring, Johann Heinrich}, title = {Facial expression as a behavioral indicator of emotional states}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-58753}, year = {1989}, abstract = {This article gives an overview of possibilities for the assessment offacial behavior. With regard to validity, results from a longitudinal study of 36 depressed patients and nine controls as weil as often schizophrenic patients and their relatives will be referred to. These results are used to illustrate the following principles which have to be taken into account when studying facial behavior: a) communication strongly facilitates facial expression, b) activation of facial behavior follows the "principle of least effort", and c) the principle of individual specificity applies to the association of nonverbal behavior and mood states. Making allowance for these principles has, among others, consequences a) for situations or conditions under which to asses behavior (specifically conditions of communication), b) for data analysis (e.g., dealing with frequent and rare events), and c) for empirical or experimental strategies (e.g., aggregation of single-case longitudinal comparisons). From the results on facial behavior during depression it can be concluded that the nonverbal reaction tendencies of endogenous and neurotic depressed patients differ. Moreover, the differential behavioral pattems observed cast doubt on the assumption of a homogeneity of affects in depression. Taking into account the conditions which govern it, facial behavior has proved to be a valid and, especially, a differential indicator for pathoIogic affective states and their changes. Given the fact that a psychiatric illness generally incorporates emotional problems it is more than surprising that little attention has been paid to the systematic study of emotional behavior. Some of the reasons for this will be clarified in the following.}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @inproceedings{BossertMeilerLaessleetal.1989, author = {Bossert, Sabine and Meiler, Caroline and Laessle, Reinhold and Ellgring, Heiner and Pirke, Karl-Martin}, title = {Responses to visual perception of food in eating disorders}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-58762}, year = {1989}, abstract = {No abstract available}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @article{CarrKurtzSchneideretal.1989, author = {Carr, Martha and Kurtz, Beth E. and Schneider, Wolfgang and Turner, Lisa A. and Borkowski, John G.}, title = {Strategy acquisition and transfer among American and German children: Environmental influences on metacognitive development}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62082}, year = {1989}, abstract = {This study explored the differential effects of strategy training on German and American elementaryschool children and assessed the role of parents in the development of their children's strategic behavior and metacognition. 184 German and 161 American children were pretested on memory and metamemory tasks. Children were then assigned to either an organizational strategy training condition or a control condition. All children were tested on the maintenance and far-transfer of the strategy and task-related metamemory 1 week following training. Parents completed questionnaires about strategy instruction in the home. Strategy maintenance and metacognition were reassessed 6 months following training. German children were more strategic than American children. Instructed children performed better than control children. German parents reported more instruction of strategies in the home. These data suggest that formal education is responsible for aspects of cognitive development that have sometimes been viewed as a function of age.}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @article{SchneiderKoerkelWeinert1989, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang and K{\"o}rkel, Joachim and Weinert, Franz E.}, title = {Domain-Specific Knowledge and Memory Performance: A Comparison of High- and Low-Aptitude Children}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62107}, year = {1989}, abstract = {Two studies compared memory performance and text comprehension of groups that were equivalent on domain-specific knowledge but differed in overall aptitude, to investigate whether prior knowledge about a particular domain or overall aptitude level was more important when the task was to acquire and use new information in the domain of interest. Both studies dealt with third-, fifth-, and seventh-grade soccer experts' and novices' memory and comprehension of a story dealing with a soccer game. Several measures of memory performance, memory monitoring, and text comprehension were used. Levels of soccer knowledge and of overall aptitude were varied in a factorial design. Neither study detected significant differences between high-aptitude and low-aptitude experts, regardless of their ages. Low aptitude experts outperformed high-aptitude novices on all memory and comprehension measures. The results indicate that domain-specific knowledge can compensate for low overall aptitude on domain-related cognitive tasks.}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @article{SchneiderKoerkel1989, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang and K{\"o}rkel, Joachim}, title = {The knowledge base and text recall: Evidence from a short-term longitudinal study}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62093}, year = {1989}, abstract = {In a short-term longitudinal study, we investigated how domain-specific knowledge in soccer influences the amount of text recall and comprehension in elementary school and junior high school children of high and low overall aptitudes. Both level of soccer knowledge and overall aptitude were varied in a factorial design. Third, fifth, and seventh grade children were given several measures of text recall and comprehension and were retested on these measures about 1 year later. Performance was more a function of soccer knowledge than of aptitude level.}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @article{BorkowskiSchneiderPressley1989, author = {Borkowski, John G. and Schneider, Wolfgang and Pressley, Michael}, title = {The challenges of teaching good information processing to learning disabled students}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62117}, year = {1989}, abstract = {A MODEL of good information processing is sketched, describing how metacognitive knowledge influences strategy selection and use. Three factors pose particular problems for learning disabled students as they attempt to acquire metacognitive knowledge and to use study strategies productively: neurological impairments; deficiencies in general world knowledge; and negative beliefs, attitudes, and styles that limit self-efficacy. Creating an educational atmosphere that explicitly builds conceptual (domain-specific) knowledge and teaches positive beliefs about learning potential is essential in promoting metacognitively-oriented instruction.}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @incollection{HommersAnderson1989, author = {Hommers, Wilfried and Anderson, Norman A.}, title = {Algebraic schemes in legal thought and in everyday morality}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-44015}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}t W{\"u}rzburg}, year = {1989}, abstract = {No abstract available}, subject = {Kriminalpsychologie / Aufsatzsammlung}, language = {en} } @incollection{Hommers1989, author = {Hommers, Wilfried}, title = {Die empirische G{\"u}ltigkeit impliziter Theorien des rechtlichen Denkens als Fragestellung der Rechtspsychologie}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-44050}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}t W{\"u}rzburg}, year = {1989}, abstract = {No abstract available}, subject = {Kognitive Psychologie / Aufsatzsammlung / Gerichtliche Psychologie / Schlagwort Klinische Psychologie}, language = {de} } @article{HommersBohnert1989, author = {Hommers, Wilfried and Bohnert, Renate}, title = {Das Urteil Geistigbehinderter {\"u}ber die Entschuldigung oder die Dritt-Entsch{\"a}digung f{\"u}r einen Diebstahl}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-69678}, year = {1989}, abstract = {Two groups of mentally retarded individuals whose averagemental age was either seven orten years, although being chronologically at least 18 years old, judged how bad "thefts" of two amounts of bon-bons appeared to them. The scimulus stories also informed the subjects whether the thief did or did not apologize afterwards or whether a third party did or did not compensate for tbe losses. Apology affecred tbe moral judgments twice as much as third-pany compensation, independeotly of the mental age of the subjects. Thus, with the mental age variation the age trend found in a prior srudy with normally gifted was not replicated. lnstead, even with an average mental age of seven years the retarded judged similarly to the adults and 10-year-olds of the prior study. However, the retarded were like the preschool children of the prior study as their judgments about the intended taking of cookies did not differ from their judgments about inadvenantly taking cookies.}, subject = {Geistig Behinderter}, language = {de} } @article{Hommers1989, author = {Hommers, Wilfried}, title = {Zur Validierung von Konfigurationstypen generalisierter Kontroll{\"u}berzeugungen}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-69686}, year = {1989}, abstract = {Die Ergebnisse einer publizierten Arbeit zur Validierung von acht Konfigurationen generalisierter Kontroll{\"u}berzeugungen werden durch drei weitere Auswertungsschritte erg{\"a}nzt. Die Anwendung der Interaktionsstrukturanalyse f{\"u}hrt zu einer Hervorhebung von zwei Konfigurationen als Typen und zur Aufstellung einer 5-Gruppen-Hypothese {\"u}ber die Bildung einer Restgruppe aus den Neutral-Konfigurationen. Die Betrachtung der Unterschiede in den FPI-Mittelwerten der urspr{\"u}nglichen acht Gruppen best{\"a}tigt die 5-Gruppen-Hypothese. Mit einer multidimensionalen geometrischen Darstellung der Profildistanzen wird in einem dritten Schritt versucht, neben der "Restgruppe" und den beiden "Typen"-Gruppen auch die Eigenst{\"a}ndigkeit der beiden verbleibenden "Anti-Typen"-Gruppen zu validieren.}, subject = {Validierung}, language = {de} }