@article{SchneiderKoerkelVogel1987, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang and K{\"o}rkel, Joachim and Vogel, Klaus}, title = {Zusammenh{\"a}nge zwischen Metaged{\"a}chtnis, strategischem Verhalten und Ged{\"a}chtnisleistungen im Grundschulalter: Eine entwicklungspsychologische Studie}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-87368}, year = {1987}, abstract = {In der vorliegenden Studie wurde die Frage gepr{\"u}ft, inwieweit Schulanf{\"a}nger im Vergleich zu fortgeschrittenen Grundsch{\"u}lern tats{\"a}chlich nur wenig dar{\"u}ber wissen, welche Strategien in freien Reproduktionsaufgaben (son-recall) voneilhaft sind. Zu diesem Zweck wurde das Metaged{\"a}chtnis von Zweit- und Vienkl{\"a}ßlern, also ihr Wissen um die Vorteile unterschiedlicher Sortier- und Lernstrategien, mit ihrem Lernverhalten bzw. ihrem Leistungsverm{\"o}gen in unterschiedlichen Varianten der sort-recall-Aufgabe verglichen. Diese Prozedur gestattete es, die Bewertung unterschiedlicher Strategien direkt mit ihrer jeweiligen Wirksamkeit zu vergleichen. Die an je 32 Zweit- und Viertkl{\"a}ßlern gewonnenen Befunde: lassen sich insgesamt so interpretieren, daß die j{\"u}ngeren Kinder kaum etwas {\"u}ber angemessene Verhaltensweisen bei freien Reproduktionsaufgaben wissen; sie ziehen perzeptuelle Organisationsstrategien konzeptuellen (taxonomischen) Organisationsstrategien vor, profitieren tats{\"a}chlich jedoch signifikant mehr von konzeptuellen Strategien. Demgegen{\"u}ber verf{\"u}gten die: Vienkl{\"a}ßler in der Regel {\"u}ber angemessenes Metaged{\"a}chtnis: sie sch{\"a}tzten taxonomische Strategien nicht nur h{\"o}her ein, sondern profitierten auch de facto deutlich mehr von solchen Techniken. Die Befunde k{\"o}nnen insgesamt als Best{\"a}tigung der in der neueren Literatur verbreiteten Annahme gewertet werden, daß sich das Wissen um Ged{\"a}chtnisanforderungen bei sort-recall-Aufgaben erst gegen Ende: der Grundschulzeit herausbildet.}, subject = {Metaged{\"a}chtnis}, language = {de} } @article{Schneider1988, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {Zur Rolle des Wissens bei kognitiven H{\"o}chstleistungen}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-87383}, year = {1988}, abstract = {Neuere Ans{\"a}tze der Hochbegabtenforschung haben demonstriert, daß herausragende intellektuelle F{\"a}higkeiten alleine noch keine kognitiven H{\"o}chstleistungen in sp{\"a}teren Lebensabschnitten garantieren. Insbesondere retrospektive Analysen der Daten genialer bzw. kognitiv hochproduktiver Pers{\"o}nlichkeiten lassen darauf schließen, daß neben Intelligenzmerkmalen auch Wissensaspekte und nichtkognitive Pers{\"o}nlichkeitsmerkmale entscheidend daf{\"u}r verantwortlich sind, daß H{\"o}chstleistungen erzielt werden k{\"o}nnen. Im vorliegenden Beitrag wird ein {\"U}berblick {\"u}ber Studien gegeben, die den Einfluß bereichsspezifiscben Wissens auf kognitive H{\"o}chstleistungen darstellt, daß jedoch ab einem bestimmten Grenzwert intellektueller F{\"a}higkeit allein das Ausmaß bereichsspezifischen Wissens in Kombination mit extremen Auspr{\"a}gungen in nichtkognitiven Merkmalen wie Konzentrationsf{\"a}higkeit, Ausdauer und Erfolgsmotivation dar{\"u}ber entscheidet, ob intellektuelle H{\"o}chstleistungen erbracht werden k{\"o}nnen.}, subject = {Hochbegabung}, language = {de} } @article{Schneider1984, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {Zur Entwicklung des Metaged{\"a}chtnisses bei Kindern}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-81004}, year = {1984}, abstract = {Neuere Untersuchungen zeigen, daß f{\"u}r die unterschiedlichen Ged{\"a}chtnisleistungen bei Kindern nicht verschieden große Ged{\"a}chtniskapazit{\"a}ten verantwortlich sind. Vielmehr k{\"o}nnen manche Kinder die zum Lernen und Erinnern n{\"o}tigen Strategien nicht effektiv genug einsetzen ; sie wissen zu wenig {\"u}ber ihr Ged{\"a}chtnis. Eine F{\"o}rderung dieses Ged{\"a}chtniswissens k{\"o}nnte ihre schulischen Leistungen erheblich verbessern.}, subject = {Metaged{\"a}chtnis}, language = {de} } @book{Schneider1989, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {Zur Entwicklung des Meta-Ged{\"a}chtnisses bei Kindern}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-87165}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}t W{\"u}rzburg}, year = {1989}, abstract = {No abstract available.}, subject = {Kind}, language = {de} } @incollection{Schneider1992, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {Zum Erwerb von Organisationsstrategien bei Kindern}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-50446}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}t W{\"u}rzburg}, year = {1992}, abstract = {No abstract available}, subject = {Lerntechnik}, language = {de} } @article{OpwisGoldGruberetal.1990, author = {Opwis, Klaus and Gold, Andreas and Gruber, Hans and Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {Zum Einfluß von Expertise auf Ged{\"a}chtnisleistungen sowie deren Selbsteinsch{\"a}tzung bei Kindern und Erwachsenen}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-86565}, year = {1990}, abstract = {Junge und erwachsene Schachexperten und -novizen wurden bez{\"u}glich ihrer Behaltensleistungen f{\"u}r kurzzeitig dargebotene Schachstellungen und f{\"u}r Anordnungen geometrischer K{\"o}rper miteinander verglichen. Die Ergebnisse zeigen eine differenzierte Wirksamkeit von Expertise in Abh{\"a}ngigkeit von der Vertrautheit mit dem zu Lernenden Material und von der Art der Aufgabenstellung. Je vorwissensbezogener das zu Lernende Material ist, desto deutlicher ist der Einfluß von Expertise auf Ged{\"a}chtnisleistungen nachweisbar. Dies gilt in gleicher Weise f{\"u}r unmittelbare wie f{\"u}r l{\"a}ngerfristige Behaltensleistungen und f{\"u}r den Lernfortschritt. Im Unterschied dazu zeigt sich weder bei der Vorhersage eigener k{\"u}nftiger noch bei der Bewertung erbrachter Ged{\"a}chtnisleistungen ein systematischer Einfluß von Expertise.}, subject = {Ged{\"a}chtnisleistung}, language = {de} } @article{Schneider1991, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {Wie wird man Spitzensportler? Entwicklungsvoraussetzungen sportlicher H{\"o}chstleistungen}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-86589}, year = {1991}, abstract = {Talentsuche und Talententwicklung ist ein altes und doch stets modernes Thema sportpsychologischer Forschung. Im folgenden Beitrag werden Ergebnisse zu den Entwicklungsvoraussetzungen intellektueller Leistungen auf den Bereich sportlicher H{\"o}chstleistung {\"u}bertragen.}, subject = {Voraussetzung}, language = {de} } @incollection{SchneiderMoebus1986, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang and M{\"o}bus, C.}, title = {Vorwort zu: Strukturmodelle f{\"u}r L{\"a}ngsschnittdaten und Zeitreihen : LISREL, Pfad- und Varianzanalysen / Claus M{\"o}bus und Wolfgang Schneider [Hrsg.]}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-50433}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}t W{\"u}rzburg}, year = {1986}, abstract = {No abstract available}, subject = {LISREL}, language = {de} } @inproceedings{WeinertKnopfSchneider1987, author = {Weinert, Franz E. and Knopf, Monika and Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {Von allgemeinen Theorien der Ged{\"a}chtnisentwicklung zur Analyse spezifischer Lern- und Erinnerungsvorg{\"a}nge}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-87578}, year = {1987}, abstract = {No abstract available.}, subject = {Lernen}, language = {de} } @incollection{PetermannHehlSchneider1977, author = {Petermann, Franz and Hehl, Franz-Josef and Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {Ver{\"a}nderungsmessung im Rahmen der klassischen Testtheorie}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-87204}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}t W{\"u}rzburg}, year = {1977}, abstract = {Der Beitrag diskutiert die Probleme der Ver{\"a}nderungsmessung im Rahmen der klassischen Testtheorie. Ausgehend von den Axiomen der klassischen Testtheorie (vgl. LoRD und NovrcK 1968) werden exemplarisch f{\"u}r den klinischen Bereich verschiedene Ver{\"a}nderungsindices abgeleitet. Diese werden zun{\"a}chst vergleichend theoretisch diskutiert. In die Darstellung fließen dann herk{\"o}mmliche Auswertungsstrategien der Ver{\"a}nderungsmessung ein (non parametrische Tests, Pfadanalyse, Cross-Lagged-Analysis, multivariate Varianzanalyse, Faktorenanalyse etc.). Eine Extension der klassischen Testtheorie stellt der Ansatz von BEREITER (1963), die Einf{\"u}hrung {\"a}nderungssensitiver Items, dar. - Alle methodischen Erw{\"a}gungen sollen m{\"o}glichst an Forschungsbeispielen der Klinischen Psychologie er{\"o}rtert werden. Abschließend sollen f{\"u}r die Forschungspraxis Handlungskonzepte erkennbar sein.}, subject = {Ver{\"a}nderungsmessung}, language = {de} } @article{SchneiderSpringerPetermann1977, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang and Springer, Adolf and Petermann, Franz}, title = {Verschl{\"u}sselung von verbaler Information bei schwachen und normalen Rechtschreibern}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-87211}, year = {1977}, abstract = {Es wird untersucht, wie Kinder unterschiedlicher Rechtschreibf{\"a}higkeit verbale Information verarbeiten. Die Hypothesen basieren auf neueren Ergebnissen der Ged{\"a}chtnisforschung, in denen die akustische Kodierung im Kurzzeitged{\"a}chtnis und die semantische Verschl{\"u}sselung im Langzeitged{\"a}thtnis lokalisiert wird. Je 35 normale und schwache Rechtschreiber des 3. und 4. Schuljahres nahmen an einem Behaltensexperiment teil. 10 Wortlisten wurden als Wiedererkennungstest vorgegeben. Die Ergebnisse zeigen, daß normale Rechtschreiber und Legastheniker voraussichtlich unterschiedliche Strategien bei der Speicherung verbaler Information anwenden.}, subject = {Rechtschreibschw{\"a}che}, language = {de} } @article{SchneiderZielinski1983, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang and Zielinski, Werner}, title = {Vergleichende Bedingungsanalysen zur Performanz guter und schwacher Rechtschreiber}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-87297}, year = {1983}, abstract = {Es wurde die Frage {\"u}berpr{\"u}ft, ob f{\"u}r schwache Rechtschreiber und rechtschreibunauff{\"a}llige Sch{\"u}ler {\"a}hnliche Determinanten der Rechtschreibleistung angenommen werden k{\"o}nnen. Theoretisch plausible Kausalmodelle zur Beschreibung und Erkl{\"a}rung von Rechtschreibleistungen rechtschreibschwacher und rechtschreibunauff{\"a}lliger Viertkl{\"a}ßler wurden im Hinblick auf ihre {\"U}bereinstimmung und Datenkompabilit{\"a}t anhand des Computerprogramms LISREL IV analysiert. F{\"u}r beide Gruppen ergaben sich unterschiedlich strukturierte L{\"o}sungen, von denen lediglich die f{\"u}r die normalen Rechtschreiber hinsichtlich der Datenanpassung und des Prozentsatzes aufgekl{\"a}rter Kriteriumsvarianz einigermaßen befriedigen konnte.}, subject = {Rechtschreibschw{\"a}che}, language = {de} } @inproceedings{SchneiderWeinert1989, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang and Weinert, Franz E.}, title = {Universal trends and individual differences in memory development}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-86518}, year = {1989}, abstract = {No abstract available.}, subject = {Ged{\"a}chtnisbildung}, language = {en} } @article{KempertGoetzBlatteretal.2016, author = {Kempert, Sebastian and G{\"o}tz, Regina and Blatter, Kristine and Tibken, Catharina and Artelt, Cordula and Schneider, Wolfgang and Stanat, Petra}, title = {Training Early Literacy Related Skills: To Which Degree Does a Musical Training Contribute to Phonological Awareness Development?}, series = {Frontiers in Psychology}, volume = {7}, journal = {Frontiers in Psychology}, number = {1803}, doi = {10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01803}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-165272}, year = {2016}, abstract = {Well-developed phonological awareness skills are a core prerequisite for early literacy development. Although effective phonological awareness training programs exist, children at risk often do not reach similar levels of phonological awareness after the intervention as children with normally developed skills. Based on theoretical considerations and first promising results the present study explores effects of an early musical training in combination with a conventional phonological training in children with weak phonological awareness skills. Using a quasi-experimental pretest-posttest control group design and measurements across a period of 2 years, we tested the effects of two interventions: a consecutive combination of a musical and a phonological training and a phonological training alone. The design made it possible to disentangle effects of the musical training alone as well the effects of its combination with the phonological training. The outcome measures of these groups were compared with the control group with multivariate analyses, controlling for a number of background variables. The sample included N = 424 German-speaking children aged 4-5 years at the beginning of the study. We found a positive relationship between musical abilities and phonological awareness. Yet, whereas the well-established phonological training produced the expected effects, adding a musical training did not contribute significantly to phonological awareness development. Training effects were partly dependent on the initial level of phonological awareness. Possible reasons for the lack of training effects in the musical part of the combination condition as well as practical implications for early literacy education are discussed.}, language = {en} } @article{BjorklundSchneiderCasseletal.1994, author = {Bjorklund, David F. and Schneider, Wolfgang and Cassel, William S. and Ashley, Elizabeth}, title = {Training and Extension of a Memory Strategy: Evidence for Utilization Deficiencies in the Acquisition of an Organizational Strategy in High- and Low-IQ Children}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62234}, year = {1994}, abstract = {143 9- and 10-year-oId children were classified into high- and Jow-IQ groups and given 4 different sort/recall lists (baseline, training, near [immediate] extension, far [l-week] extension) to assess training and extension of an organizational memory strategy. All children received categorized items of moderate typicality for Phases 1, 3, and 4. For Phase 2, children were assigned to either a training or control group, with half of the children in each group receiving category typical items and the others category atypical items. Levels of recall, sorting, and clustering were greater in Phase 2 for high-IQ children, for the typical lists, and for trained children. Both the high- and low-IQ children trained with typical items continued to show high levels of recall on the near extension phase. No group of subjects maintained high levels of recall after 1 week, although levels of sorting and/or clustering on the extension trials remained high for all groups of subjects except the low-IQ control children. This latter pattern (elevated sorting/clustering with low levels of recall) is an indication of a utilization deficiency, a phase in strategy development when children use a strategy but gain little or no benefit n performance. The results provide evidence for IQ, training, and material effects in the demonstration of a utilization deficiency.}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @article{WimmerLanderlSchneider1994, author = {Wimmer, Heinz and Landerl, Karin and Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {The role of rhyme awareness in learning to read a regular orthography}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-50508}, year = {1994}, abstract = {The present research examined whether children's awareness of phonological similarities between words with respect to rhyme and consonantal word onset is of the same importance for learning to read German as it was found to be for learning to read English. In two longitudinal studies differences in phonological sensitivity among children before learning to read (at age 6 to 7) were tested with versions of Bradley \& Bryant's (1985) oddity detection task. Children's reading and spelling achievements were tested about one year later at the end of grade one, and again at around the age of 10. The main finding was a developmental change in the predictive relationship of rhyme and word-onset awareness. Rhyme awareness was only minimally predictive for reading and spelling achievement at the end of grade one, but gained substantially in predictive importance for reading and spelling achievement in grades three and four. No such predictive improvement was observed for word-onset awareness. It is proposed that rhyme awareness is initially of little importance, because in the first phase of learning to read German children rely heavily on indirect word recognition via grapheme--phoneme translation and blending. The gain in the predictive importance of rhyme awareness is explained by its helpful effect on the establishment of mental representations of written words. Such mental representations allow fast, direct word recognition and orthographically correct spellings. A wareness of larger phonological units is helpful for the efficient establishment of such representations, by allowing connections of recurring grapheme clusters in written words with phonology.}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @article{TibkenRichterLindenetal.2022, author = {Tibken, Catharina and Richter, Tobias and Linden, Nicole von der and Schmiedeler, Sandra and Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {The role of metacognitive competences in the development of school achievement among gifted adolescents}, series = {Child Development}, volume = {93}, journal = {Child Development}, number = {1}, doi = {10.1111/cdev.13640}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-258376}, pages = {117-133}, year = {2022}, abstract = {Gifted underachievers perform worse in school than would be expected based on their high intelligence. Possible causes for underachievement are low motivational dispositions (need for cognition) and metacognitive competences. This study tested the interplay of these variables longitudinally with gifted and non-gifted students from Germany (N = 341, 137 females) in Grades 6 (M = 12.02 years at t1) and 8 (M = 14.07 years). Declarative and procedural metacognitive competences were assessed in the domain of reading comprehension. Path analyses showed incremental effects of procedural metacognition over and above intelligence on the development of school achievement in gifted students (β = .139). Moreover, declarative metacognition and need for cognition interactively predicted procedural metacognition (β = .169), which mediated their effect on school achievement.}, language = {en} } @inproceedings{SchneiderWeinert1990, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang and Weinert, Franz E.}, title = {The role of knowledge, strategies, and aptitudes in cognitive Performance: Concluding Comments}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-87190}, year = {1990}, abstract = {No abstract available.}, subject = {Kognitive Entwicklung}, language = {en} } @inproceedings{SchneiderWeinert1990, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang and Weinert, Franz E.}, title = {The role of knowledge, strategies, and aptitudes in cognitive performance : concluding comments}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-86554}, year = {1990}, abstract = {No abstract available.}, subject = {Intelligenzleistung}, language = {en} } @incollection{BjorklundMuirBroaddusSchneider1990, author = {Bjorklund, David F. and Muir-Broaddus, Jacqueline E. and Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {The role of knowledge in the development of strategies}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-86538}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}t W{\"u}rzburg}, year = {1990}, abstract = {No abstract available.}, subject = {Wissen}, language = {en} } @article{BjorklundSchneiderHarnishfegeretal.1992, author = {Bjorklund, David F. and Schneider, Wolfgang and Harnishfeger, Katherine Kipp and Cassel, William S. and Bjorklund, Barbara R. and Bernholtz, Jean E.}, title = {The role of IQ, expertise, and motivation in the recall of familiar Information}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62204}, year = {1992}, abstract = {High- and low-IQ children in the first, third, and fifth grades performed two free-recall tasks: a sort-recall task with sets of categorically related pictures, and a class-recall task, with children recalling the current members of their school class. All children were deemed to be experts concerning the composition of their school class, but, unlike experts in other domains, had no special motivation associated with their expertise. Recall and clustering on both tasks were high. The high-IQ children performed better than low-IQ children only on the sort-recall task. IQ was significantly correlated with measures of performance on the sort-recall task but not on the class-recall task. The results reflect the fact that the memory benefits associated with being an expert (here, elimination of IQ effects) are related to the greater knowledge the expert possesses and not to factors of motivation.}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @article{SchneiderBrun1987, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang and Brun, Hedwig}, title = {The role of context in young children's memory performance: Istomina revisited}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-52726}, year = {1987}, abstract = {According to more recent studies on memory development in young children, preschoolers and kindergarteners are able to demonstrate surprisingly good memory skills in natural as weH as in laboratory-type settings. This finding is not consistent with the results of a study by Istomina (1975), conducted in 1948, leading to the concJusion that (a) preschoolers do not use voluntary remembering, and (b) children generally recall better in play situations than in typical experimental settings. In this study, two experiments were conducted to replicate Istomina's research. In the first, it was shown that Istomina's findings were replicable when methodological problems in the procedure were ignored. Experiment 2 improved methodologically upon Istomina's experimental methods and did not produce results to support her concJusions. Four- and 6-year-olds showed voluntary memory in play activities as weH as in laboratory-type settings, and remembered equally weB in both contexts. The results did not support the assumption that memory performance in young children can be substantially facilitated by motivating contexts.}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @article{Schneider1986, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {The role of conceptual knowledge and metamemory in the development of organizational processes in memory}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62022}, year = {1986}, abstract = {The present study investigated the relationshtp between developmental shifts in the organization of materials and developmental changes in deliberate strategy use. Second and fourth grade children were presented with clusterable sort/recall lists representing the factorial combinations of high and low interitem association, and high and low category relatedness. Strategy use in the task was rated by the experimenter and also assessed via self reports. General and task-related strategy knowledge tmetamemoryt was also examined. Second graders displayed more category clustering during recall for highly associated items than for weakly associated items. whereas older children's recall organization (but not recall) was unaffected by this organizational dimension. Correlations among measures of metamemory and organizational behavior indicated that second graders in general were unaware of the importance of categorization strategies for facilitation of recall. On the other hand. sorting during study and task-related metamemory were the most important predictors of fourth graders' recall performance, thus indicating that most fourth graders used categorization strategies deliberately.}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @article{SchneiderHelmke1986, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang and Helmke, Andreas}, title = {The role of classroom differences in achievement changes}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-87356}, year = {1986}, abstract = {A combined criterion involving the regression slopes of pretest-posttest achievement scores and achievement gain scores was used to classify similar types of classrooms. Mathematics achievement differences among 632 fifth graders were analysed in a longitudinal design and explained in a structural equation framework provided by LISREL, separately for four types of classrooms. The results replicated the findings of an earlier study (Schneider \& Treiber, 1984) in that the local nature of achievement models could be demonstrated. That is, the structural components of the causal models could not be generalized across the four groups of classrooms. The inclusion of a second grouping criterion (i. e., achievement gainJ proved useful in that a better model fit was always obtained for classrooms with high achievement gains. As a global model test ignoring group and classroom membership did mask the differential validity of the achievement model in the various subgro.ups, the need for multilevel approaches was emphasized.}, subject = {Schulleistung}, language = {en} } @article{GaultneyBjorklundSchneider1992, author = {Gaultney, Jane F. and Bjorklund, David F. and Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {The role of children's expertise in a Strategie memory task}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62190}, year = {1992}, abstract = {In a study intended to replicate and extend the findings from a recent experiment by Schneider and Bjorklund (1992), the expert/novice paradigm was used with second- and fourth-grade children in a sort/recall task. Children were classified as experts or novices for their knowledge of baseball, then given two sort/recall tasks, with a list consisting of either baseball or nonbaseball terms. Experts recalled more than novices on the baseball list only. While both groups used organizational strategies at sorting on the nonbaseball list, experts were marginally more strategic than novices on the baseball list, and no differences were found between the groups on either list for clustering. Baseball experts used more adultlike categories, suggesting that their enhanced levels of recall were attributed in part to strategy use, although there was also evidence that most of the substantial recall difference between the groups was attributed to item-specific effects associated with a more elaborated knowledge base. A second experiment using fifth-grade children on a multitrial sort/recall task using the baseball list also found increased recall by experts, and also found evidence of strategic behavior at the sort phase for trials 3 and 4.}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @incollection{Schneider1993, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {The longitudinal study of motor development: methodological issues}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-71393}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}t W{\"u}rzburg}, year = {1993}, abstract = {No abstract available.}, subject = {Motorische Entwicklung}, language = {en} } @article{SchneiderKoerkel1989, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang and K{\"o}rkel, Joachim}, title = {The knowledge base and text recall: Evidence from a short-term longitudinal study}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62093}, year = {1989}, abstract = {In a short-term longitudinal study, we investigated how domain-specific knowledge in soccer influences the amount of text recall and comprehension in elementary school and junior high school children of high and low overall aptitudes. Both level of soccer knowledge and overall aptitude were varied in a factorial design. Third, fifth, and seventh grade children were given several measures of text recall and comprehension and were retested on these measures about 1 year later. Performance was more a function of soccer knowledge than of aptitude level.}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @article{SchneiderNaeslund1993, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang and N{\"a}slund, Jan Carol}, title = {The impact of early metalinguistic competencies and memory capacity on reading and spelling in elementary school: Results of the Munich Longitudinal Study on the Genesis of Individual Competencies (LOGIC)}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-87421}, year = {1993}, abstract = {This paper reports on a longitudinal study dealing with the development of literacy in young children. A total of 163 children were first tested during their last year in kindergarten using a variety of tasks that tapped phonological processing, memory capacity, early literacy, and intelligence. Children's ward decoding, reading comprehension, and spelling skills were assessed in elementary school several years later. As a main result, all of the predictor domains had a significant impact on the acquisition of literacy in elementary school, although the contribution of each domain differed as a function of the criterion measure. An attempt to identify children-at-risk using a kindergarten screening test provided encouraging results. Nonetheless, it was shown that whereas group predictions of reading and spelling performance can be quite accurate, the individual prognosis of school problems is far from perfect.}, subject = {Lese- und Schreibf{\"a}higkeit}, language = {en} } @article{SchneiderKoerkelWeinert1987, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang and K{\"o}rkel, Joachim and Weinert, Franz E.}, title = {The effects of intelligence, self-concept, and attributional style on metamemory and memory behaviour}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62050}, year = {1987}, abstract = {No abstract available}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @article{KurtzSchneider1988, author = {Kurtz, Beth E. and Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {The effects of age, study time, and importance of text units on strategy use and memory for texts}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-87408}, year = {1988}, abstract = {This study investigated study behavior and recall of a narrative text as a function of the reader's age, study time, and importance level of text units. Fifth graders, seventh graders, young- and older adults were asked to read a fairy tale, and do anything they liked to prepare for verbatim recall. Half of the subjects in each age group were assigned to an immediate recall condition; half were given additional study time. Examination of recall data showed that all subjects showed higher recall of important units in the text than unimportant units. This effect was independent of age and study time condition. Study behaviors varied significantly across age groups and study conditions: while adults underlined or took notes with equal frequency, children preferred note-taking as a study strategy. With additional study time, fifth graders, seventh graders, and older adults increased their strategic behavior; young adults did not.}, subject = {Studienzeit}, language = {en} } @article{BorkowskiSchneiderPressley1989, author = {Borkowski, John G. and Schneider, Wolfgang and Pressley, Michael}, title = {The challenges of teaching good information processing to learning disabled students}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62117}, year = {1989}, abstract = {A MODEL of good information processing is sketched, describing how metacognitive knowledge influences strategy selection and use. Three factors pose particular problems for learning disabled students as they attempt to acquire metacognitive knowledge and to use study strategies productively: neurological impairments; deficiencies in general world knowledge; and negative beliefs, attitudes, and styles that limit self-efficacy. Creating an educational atmosphere that explicitly builds conceptual (domain-specific) knowledge and teaches positive beliefs about learning potential is essential in promoting metacognitively-oriented instruction.}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @inproceedings{Schneider1992, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {The "what" and "how" of development: Really two separate research agendas?}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-86630}, year = {1992}, abstract = {No abstract available.}, subject = {Entwicklungspsychologie}, language = {en} } @incollection{Schneider1977, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {Taxonomie der Ged{\"a}chtnisleistungen schwacher und normaler Rechtschreiber}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-69476}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}t W{\"u}rzburg}, year = {1977}, abstract = {No abstract available.}, subject = {Ged{\"a}chtnisleistung}, language = {de} } @incollection{Schneider1986, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {Strukturgleichungsmodelle der zweiten Generation - Eine Einf{\"u}hrung}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-50401}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}t W{\"u}rzburg}, year = {1986}, subject = {LISREL}, language = {de} } @article{KurtzSchneiderCarretal.1990, author = {Kurtz, Beth E. and Schneider, Wolfgang and Carr, Martha and Borkowski, John G. and Rellinger, Elizabeth}, title = {Strategy instruction and attributional beliefs in West Germany and the United States: Do teachers foster metacognitive development?}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62145}, year = {1990}, abstract = {Previous research has shown German children to be more strategic on sort-recall memory tasks than their American age-mates, and to show fewer effort-related attributions. We conducted this study to determine if those differences are due to systematic differences in the strategy instruction and attributional beliefs of German and U.S. teachers, and to explore metacognitive instructional practices in the two countries. Teachers responded to a questionnaire that inquired about their use of strategy instructions, fostering of reflective thinking in pupils, sources of children's learning problems, and modeling of metacognitive skills such as monitoring. The second part of the questionnaire asked about the reasons underlying children's academic successes and failures. German teachers reported more instruction of task-specific strategies, while American teachers showed more effort-related attributions. The types of strategies instructed and types of learning problems most frequently described varied across the two countries, and also according to how many years the teachers had taught. Results were discussed regarding their implications for metacognitive developmental theory, particularly regarding culture and other environmental influences on the development of controlled processing.}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @article{CarrKurtzSchneideretal.1989, author = {Carr, Martha and Kurtz, Beth E. and Schneider, Wolfgang and Turner, Lisa A. and Borkowski, John G.}, title = {Strategy acquisition and transfer among American and German children: Environmental influences on metacognitive development}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62082}, year = {1989}, abstract = {This study explored the differential effects of strategy training on German and American elementaryschool children and assessed the role of parents in the development of their children's strategic behavior and metacognition. 184 German and 161 American children were pretested on memory and metamemory tasks. Children were then assigned to either an organizational strategy training condition or a control condition. All children were tested on the maintenance and far-transfer of the strategy and task-related metamemory 1 week following training. Parents completed questionnaires about strategy instruction in the home. Strategy maintenance and metacognition were reassessed 6 months following training. German children were more strategic than American children. Instructed children performed better than control children. German parents reported more instruction of strategies in the home. These data suggest that formal education is responsible for aspects of cognitive development that have sometimes been viewed as a function of age.}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @inproceedings{KurtzSchneiderBorkowskietal.1988, author = {Kurtz, Beth and Schneider, Wolfgang and Borkowski, John G. and Carr, Martha and Turner, Lisa A.}, title = {Sources of memory and metamemory development: Societal, parental, and educational influences}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-50524}, year = {1988}, abstract = {This project had two goals: (1) to examine the impact of strategy training on memory performance in German and American children, and (2) to search for environmental correlates of individual differences in cognitive processes. Following pretesting, 437 children were divided into training and control groups, with the former receiving training in clustering strategies. Trained children showed sizable strategy maintenance and transfer effects two weeks and six months later. Parents and teachers completed questionnaires about the teaching of strategies and their attributional beliefs about children's academic successes and failures. The differences in strategie behavior and attributions of German and American children were due, in part, to differences in strategy-enriched environments.}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @incollection{SchneiderHasselhorn1994, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang and Hasselhorn, Marcus}, title = {Situational context features and memory development : insights from replications of Istomina's experiment}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-50397}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}t W{\"u}rzburg}, year = {1994}, abstract = {No abstract available}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @article{PressleyCarigliaBullDeaneetal.1987, author = {Pressley, Michael and Cariglia-Bull, Teresa and Deane, Shelley and Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {Short-term memory, verbal competence, and age as predictors of imagery instructional effectiveness}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62046}, year = {1987}, abstract = {No abstract available}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @article{KurtzCostesSchneider1994, author = {Kurtz-Costes, Beth E. and Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {Self-concept, attributional beliefs, and school achievement: A longitudinal analysis}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62245}, year = {1994}, abstract = {No abstract available}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @article{TreiberSchneider1981, author = {Treiber, Bernhard and Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {Schulisches Lernen im sozial{\"o}kologischen Kontext}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-87271}, year = {1981}, abstract = {No abstract available.}, subject = {Lernen}, language = {de} } @article{Schneider1981, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {Rechtschreibdiagnose in der Grundschule - Theoretische und empirische Analysen zur spezifischen Problematik}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-50535}, year = {1981}, abstract = {Theoretische Analysen zum Problem des Recht-Schreibens weisen darauf hin, daß weniger Intelligenzmerkmale als vielmehr Ged{\"a}chtnis'eistungen bei dem Erwerb der Schriftsprache von Bedeutung sind. Daraus folgt, daß f{\"u}r die Pr{\"u}fw{\"o}rter in normierten Rechtschreibtests zumindest hinreichende Vorkommensfrequenz gew{\"a}hrleistet sein sollte. Diese Frage wird in der vorliegenden Untersuchung am Beispiel des Allgemeinen Schulleistungstests {\"u}berpr{\"u}ft: f{\"u}r die ausgew{\"a}hlten Klassenstufen kann gezeigt INerden, daß die Vorkommensh{\"a}ufigkeit der Pr{\"u}fW{\"O}fter nicht ausreicht und damit wenig geeignet ist, um die tats{\"a}chliche Rechtschreibfertigkeit zu erfassen. Der Vergleich mit mehreren eng am Curriculum orientierten Diktatproben kann gleichzeitig die Schwierigkeiten verdeutlichen, die dann entstehen, wenn zuverl{\"a}ssige Bestimmungen der individuellen Rechtschreibkompetenz vorgenommen werden sollen.}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {de} } @article{SodianSchneiderPerlmutter1986, author = {Sodian, Beate and Schneider, Wolfgang and Perlmutter, Marion}, title = {Recall, clustering, and metamemory in young children}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62014}, year = {1986}, abstract = {Thirty-two 4-year-olds and thirty-two 6-year-olds were tested for free and cued recall following either play-and-remember or sort-and-remember instructions and assessed for their metamemory of the efficacy of conceptual and perceptual sorting strategies. The younger children recalled significantly more items under sort-and-remember than under play-and-remember instructions, whereas no significant recall differences between instructional conditions were found for the older children. However, 6-year-olds showed higher levels of recall than 4-year-olds in both instructional conditions. Category cues were much more effective than color cues, regardless of age. In addition, clustering scores indicated that conceptual organization at both encoding and retrieval increased with age and with instruction. These results show that from 4 to 6 years of age children are learning to spontaneously employ memory strategies. In addition, they highlight the increasing importance of conceptual organization to retention of young children. Finally, the metamemory data suggest that there may be a lag between children's articulated declarative knowledge about the usefulness of conceptual organization and their procedural use of it.}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {en} } @article{TreiberSchneider1980, author = {Treiber, Bernhard and Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {Qualifizierende und varianzreduzierende Wirkungen von Schulklassenunterricht}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-87259}, year = {1980}, abstract = {Schulklassenunterricht soll m{\"o}glichst vielen Sch{\"u}lern ein m{\"o}glichst hohes kognitives Leistungsniveau erreichen helfen. Nur wenige Unterrichtsstudien haben bisher jedoch zur Aufkl{\"a}rung jener Lehr-Lern-Bedingungen beigerragen, unter denen sich hohe qualifikatorische und varianzreduktive Wirkungen gleichzeitig erreichen lassen. Die vorliegende Studie greift daf{\"u}r auf Merkmale einer adaptiv-remedialen Lehr-Lern-Organisation zur{\"u}ck und {\"u}berpr{\"u}ft ihren Erkl{\"a}rungsgehalt im Mathematikunterricht von 58 f{\"u}nften Hauptschulklassen. Zur systematischen Entwicklung komplexer Erkl{\"a}rungen und ihrer einzelfallartigen Beurteilung wird ein neu entwickeltes Programmsystem (HYPAG) angewandt. Es f{\"u}hn zur Auswahl einer Annahmenstruktur, in der Instrumental- und Rahmenbedingungen von Schulklassenunterricht in Wechselwirkung stehen, f{\"u}r sich genommen aber nur mehr hinreichende und darin substiruierbare Bedingungen qualifizierender und varianzreduktiver Unterrichtseffekte sind.}, subject = {Schulklasse}, language = {de} } @inproceedings{Schneider1989, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {Problems of longitudinal studies with young children: Practical, conceptual, and methodological issues}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-86521}, year = {1989}, abstract = {No abstract available.}, subject = {L{\"a}ngsschnittuntersuchung}, language = {en} } @article{SchneiderScheibler1983, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang and Scheibler, D.}, title = {Probleme und M{\"o}glichkeiten bei der Bewertung von Clusteranalysen: I. Ein {\"U}berblick {\"u}ber einschl{\"a}gige Evaluationsstudien}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-87288}, year = {1983}, abstract = {Es wird ein Oberblick {\"u}ber Evaluationsstudien gegeben, die sich mit der Validit{\"a}t von Clusteranalyse-Algorithmen befassen. Im Anschluß an die Diskussion m{\"o}glicher Bewertungskriterien werden Vergleichsuntersuchungen n{\"a}her analysiert und danach geordnet, ob sie empirische Datens{\"a}tze, Plasmaden oder Monte-Carlo-Datens{\"a}tze als Evaluationsgrundlage benutzen. Die Obersicht {\"u}ber komplexer angelegte Monte-Carlo-Studien zeigt die unterschiedliche Qualit{\"a}t der verf{\"u}gbaren Clusteranalyse-Algorithmen auf, macht andererseits aber auch deutlich, daß bestimmte hierarchisch-agglomerative Verfahren wie etwa die Methoden nachWARD oder LANCE-WILLIAMS bzw. iterativpartitionierende Prozeduren wie etwa die KMEANS-Algorithmen als relativ robuste Klassifikationsverfahren gelten k{\"o}nnen.}, subject = {Cluster-Analyse}, language = {de} } @article{SchneiderScheibler1983, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang and Scheibler, D.}, title = {Probleme und M{\"o}glichkeiten bei der Bewertung von Clusteranalyse-Verfahren: III. Appendix: Kurzbeschreibung der verbreitetsten Clusteranalyse-Algorithmen}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-69643}, year = {1983}, abstract = {Es wird eine relativ einfach gehaltene Kurzcharakteristik derjenigen Clusteranalyse-Algorithmen gegeben, die aufgrund eines Literatur{\"u}berblicks (SCHNEIDER \& SCHEIBLER 1983a) als die in der Fonchung haupts{\"a}chlich benutzten Verfahren einzustufen sind. Die Kurzbeschreibung verzichtet im wesentlichen auf statistische Details und verfolgt speziell das Ziel, dem Leser eine Vorstellung von Gemeinsamkeiten und Untenchieden in der Funktionsweise von hierarchischen Clusteranalysen, Optimierungs- bzw. Partitionierungstechniken, Dichteverfahren, "Clumping Techniques" und anderen Prozeduren zu geben.}, subject = {Cluster-Analyse}, language = {de} } @article{SchneiderScheibler1983, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang and Scheibler, D.}, title = {Probleme und M{\"o}glichkeiten bei der Bewertung von Clusteranalyse-Verfahren: II. Ergebnisse einer Monte-Carlo-Studie}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-69637}, year = {1983}, abstract = {Ziel der vorliegenden Untersuchung war es, Aufschluß {\"u}ber die unterschiedliche Qualit{\"a}t hierarchischer und nicht-hierarchischer (partionierender) Clusteranalyseverfahren zu gewinnen. Die Reproduktionsg{\"u}te beider Clusteranalyse-Varianten wurde anhand von 200 Monte-Carlo-Datens{\"a}tzen (multivariat normalverteilte Mixturen) zu {\"u}berpr{\"u}fen versucht, wobei jeweils unterschiedliche Proportionen der Daten-Elemente klassifiZiert werden mußten. Es zeigte sich, daß insgesamt gesehen die hierarchischen Algorithmen nach WARD und LANCE-WILUAMS am besten dazu in der Lage waren, die vorgegebenen Datenstrukturen zu reproduzieren, andererseits aber die herangezogenen partitionierenden KMEANS-Verfahren nicht schlechter abschnitten, wenn die L{\"o}sung der WARD-Technik als Start-Partition vorgegeben wurde.}, subject = {Monte-Carlo-Simulation}, language = {de} } @article{Schneider1982, author = {Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {Neuere Trends der Rechtschreibforschung}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-50475}, year = {1982}, abstract = {No abstract available}, subject = {Psychologie}, language = {de} } @article{OpwisGoldSchneider1987, author = {Opwis, K. and Gold, A. and Schneider, Wolfgang}, title = {M{\"o}glichkeiten der Kreuzvalidierung von Strukturgleichungsmodellen}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-87377}, year = {1987}, abstract = {Die Beurteilung der Angemessenheit theoretischer Oberlegungen auf der Grundlage statistischer Hypothesentests ist f{\"u}r die empirische Forschung von zentraler Bedeutung. Im Mittelpunkt der Arbeit stehen die mit der Testung von multivariaten Strukturgleichungsmodellen (LISREL-Modelle) verbundenen Probleme. Zu Beginn wird das LISREL-Modell unter den Aspekten der simultanen Analyse von Kovarianzund Mittelwertstrukturen und der Modelltestung diskutiert. Anschließend werden anhand eines Illustrationsbeispiels aus der p{\"a}dagogisch-psychologischen Forschung verschiedene Vorgehensweisen der Kreuzvalidierung einander gegen{\"u}bergestellt und an empirischen Daten demonstriert. Es zeigt sich, daß die Obernahme exakter numerischer Parameterwerte sowohl aufgrund inhaltlicher wie auch formaler Argumente unangemessen streng erscheint. Diese Einsch{\"a}tzung kann empirisch belegt werden, indem eine Approximation von Intervallrestriktionen vorgenommen wird.}, subject = {Kreuzvalidierung}, language = {de} }