TY - JOUR A1 - Andreatta, Marta A1 - Mühlberger, Andreas A1 - Glotzbach-Schoon, Evelyn A1 - Pauli, Paul T1 - Pain predictability reverses valence ratings of a relief-associated stimulus JF - Front in Systems Neuroscience N2 - Relief from pain is positively valenced and entails reward-like properties. Notably, stimuli that became associated with pain relief elicit reward-like implicit responses too, but are explicitly evaluated by humans as aversive. Since the unpredictability of pain makes pain more aversive, this study examined the hypotheses that the predictability of pain also modulates the valence of relief-associated stimuli. In two studies, we presented one conditioned stimulus \((_{FORWARD}CS+)\) before a painful unconditioned stimulus (US), another stimulus \((_{BACKWARD}CS+)\) after the painful US, and a third stimulus (CS−) was never associated with the US. In Study 1, \(_{FORWARD}CS+\) predicted half of the USs while the other half was delivered unwarned and followed by \(_{BACKWARD}CS+\). In Study 2, all USs were predicted by \(_{FORWARD}CS+\) and followed by \(_{BACKWARD}CS+\). In Study 1 both \(_{FORWARD}CS+\) and \(_{BACKWARD}CS+\) were rated as negatively valenced and high arousing after conditioning, while \(_{BACKWARD}CS+\) in Study 2 acquired positive valence and low arousal. Startle amplitude was significantly attenuated to \(_{BACKWARD}CS+\) compared to \(_{FORWARD}CS+\) in Study 2, but did not differ among CSs in Study 1. In summary, predictability of aversive events reverses the explicit valence of a relief-associated stimulus. KW - threat unpredictability KW - implicit and explicit responses KW - forward conditioning KW - backward conditioning KW - pain relief Y1 - 2013 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-129275 VL - 7 IS - 53 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Schwarz, Katharina A. A1 - Wieser, Matthias J. A1 - Gerdes, Antje B. M. A1 - Mühlberger, Andreas A1 - Pauli, Paul T1 - Why are you looking like that? How the context influences evaluation and processing of human faces JF - Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience N2 - Perception and evaluation of facial expressions are known to be heavily modulated by emotional features of contextual information. Such contextual effects, however, might also be driven by non-emotional aspects of contextual information, an interaction of emotional and non-emotional factors, and by the observers’ inherent traits. Therefore, we sought to assess whether contextual information about self-reference in addition to information about valence influences the evaluation and neural processing of neutral faces. Furthermore, we investigated whether social anxiety moderates these effects. In the present functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study, participants viewed neutral facial expressions preceded by a contextual sentence conveying either positive or negative evaluations about the participant or about somebody else. Contextual influences were reflected in rating and fMRI measures, with strong effects of self-reference on brain activity in the medial prefrontal cortex and right fusiform gyrus. Additionally, social anxiety strongly affected the response to faces conveying negative, self-related evaluations as revealed by the participants’ rating patterns and brain activity in cortical midline structures and regions of interest in the left and right middle frontal gyrus. These results suggest that face perception and processing are highly individual processes influenced by emotional and non-emotional aspects of contextual information and further modulated by individual personality traits. KW - social anxiety KW - facial expression KW - context KW - self-reference Y1 - 2013 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-132126 VL - 8 IS - 4 ER - TY - THES A1 - Sollfrank, Teresa T1 - Feedback efficiency and training effects during alpha band modulation over the sensorimotor cortex T1 - Die Wirksamkeit von Feedback und Trainingseffekten während der Alphaband Modulation über dem menschlichen sensomotorischen Cortex N2 - Neural oscillations can be measured by electroencephalography (EEG) and these oscillations can be characterized by their frequency, amplitude and phase. The mechanistic properties of neural oscillations and their synchronization are able to explain various aspects of many cognitive functions such as motor control, memory, attention, information transfer across brain regions, segmentation of the sensory input and perception (Arnal and Giraud, 2012). The alpha band frequency is the dominant oscillation in the human brain. This oscillatory activity is found in the scalp EEG at frequencies around 8-13 Hz in all healthy adults (Makeig et al., 2002) and considerable interest has been generated in exploring EEG alpha oscillations with regard to their role in cognitive (Klimesch et al., 1993; Hanselmayr et al., 2005), sensorimotor (Birbaumer, 2006; Sauseng et al., 2009) and physiological (Lehmann, 1971; Niedermeyer, 1997; Kiyatkin, 2010) aspects of human life. The ability to voluntarily regulate the alpha amplitude can be learned with neurofeedback training and offers the possibility to control a brain-computer interface (BCI), a muscle independent interaction channel. BCI research is predominantly focused on the signal processing, the classification and the algorithms necessary to translate brain signals into control commands than on the person interacting with the technical system. The end-user must be properly trained to be able to successfully use the BCI and factors such as task instructions, training, and especially feedback can therefore play an important role in learning to control a BCI (Neumann and Kübler, 2003; Pfurtscheller et al., 2006, 2007; Allison and Neuper, 2010; Friedrich et al., 2012; Kaufmann et al., 2013; Lotte et al., 2013). The main purpose of this thesis was to investigate how end-users can efficiently be trained to perform alpha band modulation recorded over their sensorimotor cortex. The herein presented work comprises three studies with healthy participants and participants with schizophrenia focusing on the effects of feedback and training time on cortical activation patterns and performance. In the first study, the application of a realistic visual feedback to support end-users in developing a concrete feeling of kinesthetic motor imagery was tested in 2D and 3D visualization modality during a single training session. Participants were able to elicit the typical event-related desynchronisation responses over sensorimotor cortex in both conditions but the most significant decrease in the alpha band power was obtained following the three-dimensional realistic visualization. The second study strengthen the hypothesis that an enriched visual feedback with information about the quality of the input signal supports an easier approach for motor imagery based BCI control and can help to enhance performance. Significantly better performance levels were measurable during five online training sessions in the groups with enriched feedback as compared to a conventional simple visual feedback group, without significant differences in performance between the unimodal (visual) and multimodal (auditory–visual) feedback modality. Furthermore, the last study, in which people with schizophrenia participated in multiple sessions with simple feedback, demonstrated that these patients can learn to voluntarily regulate their alpha band. Compared to the healthy group they required longer training times and could not achieve performance levels as high as the control group. Nonetheless, alpha neurofeedback training lead to a constant increase of the alpha resting power across all 20 training session. To date only little is known about the effects of feedback and training time on BCI performance and cortical activation patterns. The presented work contributes to the evidence that healthy individuals can benefit from enriched feedback: A realistic presentation can support participants in getting a concrete feeling of motor imagery and enriched feedback, which instructs participants about the quality of their input signal can give support while learning to control the BCI. This thesis demonstrates that people with schizophrenia can learn to gain control of their alpha oscillations recorded over the sensorimotor cortex when participating in sufficient training sessions. In conclusion, this thesis improved current motor imagery BCI feedback protocols and enhanced our understanding of the interplay between feedback and BCI performance. N2 - Die Wirksamkeit von Feedback und Trainingseffekten während der Alphaband Modulation über dem menschlichen sensomotorischen Cortex KW - Neurofeedback KW - Gehirn-Computer-Schnittstelle KW - Brain-computer interface KW - Übung KW - Alpha Neurofeedback KW - EEG Y1 - 2015 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-131769 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Fischer, Rico A1 - Plessow, Franziska A1 - Kiesel, Andrea T1 - The effects of alerting signals in masked priming JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - Alerting signals often serve to reduce temporal uncertainty by predicting the time of stimulus onset. The resulting response time benefits have often been explained by facilitated translation of stimulus codes into response codes on the basis of established stimulus-response (S-R) links. In paradigms of masked S-R priming alerting signals also modulate response activation processes triggered by subliminally presented prime stimuli. In the present study we tested whether facilitation of visuo-motor translation processes due to alerting signals critically depends on established S-R links. Alerting signals resulted in significantly enhanced masked priming effects for masked prime stimuli that included and that did not include established S-R links fi.e., target vs. novel primes). Yet, the alerting-priming interaction was more pronounced for target than for novel primes. These results suggest that effects of alerting signals on masked priming are especially evident when S-R links between prime and target exist. At the same time, an alerting-priming interaction also for novel primes suggests that alerting signals also facilitate stimulus-response translation processes when masked prime stimuli provide action-trigger conditions in terms of programmed S-R links. KW - response selection KW - visual cortex KW - temporal predictability KW - stimuli KW - primes KW - target primes KW - simon task KW - automaticity KW - performance KW - perception KW - mechanism KW - novel primes KW - action-trigger KW - masked priming KW - accessory KW - alerting signal Y1 - 2013 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-122581 SN - 1664-1078 VL - 4 IS - 448 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Meule, Adrian A1 - Kübler, Andrea A1 - Blechert, Jens T1 - Time course of electrocortical food-cue responses during cognitive regulation of craving JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - In our current obesogenic environment, exposure to visual food-cues can easily lead to craving and overeating because short-term, pleasurable effects of food intake dominate over the anticipated long-term adverse effects such as weight gain and associated health problems. Here we contrasted these two conditions during food-cue presentation while acquiring event-related potentials (ERPs) and subjective craving ratings. Female participants (n = 25) were presented with either high-calorie (HC) or low-calorie (LC) food images under instructions to imagine either immediate (NOW) or long-term effects (LATER) of consumption. On subjective ratings for HC foods, the LATER perspective reduced cravings as compared to the NOW perspective. For LC foods, by contrast, craving increased under the LATER perspective. Early ERPs (occipital N1, 150-200 ms) were sensitive to food type but not to perspective. Late ERPs (late positive potential, LPP, 350-550 ms) were larger in the HC-LATER condition than in all other conditions, possibly indicating that a cognitive focus on negative long-term consequences induced negative arousal. This enhancement for HC-LATER attenuated to the level of the LC conditions during the later slow wave (550-3000 ms), but amplitude in the HC-NOW condition was larger than in all other conditions, possibly due to a delayed appetitive response. Across all conditions, LPP amplitudes were positively correlated with self-reported emotional eating. In sum, results reveal that regulation effects are secondary to an early attentional analysis of food type and dynamically evolve over time. Adopting a long-term perspective on eating might promote a healthier food choice across a range of food types. KW - EEG KW - disorder examination questionnaire KW - eating disorder KW - emotion regulation KW - future directions KW - attention KW - brain KW - high-calorie KW - german version KW - bulimia nervosa KW - chocolate images KW - event-related potentials KW - eating KW - calorie content KW - food-cues KW - food craving KW - LPP KW - slow wave Y1 - 2013 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-122566 SN - 1664-1078 VL - 4 IS - 669 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Ehrenfeld, Stephan A1 - Herbort, Oliver A1 - Butz, Martin V. T1 - Modular neuron-based body estimation: maintaining consistency over different limbs, modalities, and frames of reference JF - Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience N2 - This paper addresses the question of how the brain maintains a probabilistic body state estimate over time from a modeling perspective. The neural Modular Modality Frame (nMMF) model simulates such a body state estimation process by continuously integrating redundant, multimodal body state information sources. The body state estimate itself is distributed over separate, but bidirectionally interacting modules. nMMF compares the incoming sensory and present body state information across the interacting modules and fuses the information sources accordingly. At the same time, nMMF enforces body state estimation consistency across the modules. nMMF is able to detect conflicting sensory information and to consequently decrease the influence of implausible sensor sources on the fly. In contrast to the previously published Modular Modality Frame (MMF) model, nMMF offers a biologically plausible neural implementation based on distributed, probabilistic population codes. Besides its neural plausibility, the neural encoding has the advantage of enabling (a) additional probabilistic information flow across the separate body state estimation modules and (b) the representation of arbitrary probability distributions of a body state. The results show that the neural estimates can detect and decrease the impact of false sensory information, can propagate conflicting information across modules, and can improve overall estimation accuracy due to additional module interactions. Even bodily illusions, such as the rubber hand illusion, can be simulated with nMMF. We conclude with an outlook on the potential of modeling human data and of invoking goal-directed behavioral control. KW - information KW - posterior parietal cortex KW - hand KW - population code KW - conflicting information KW - multimodal interaction KW - probabilistic inference KW - modular body schema KW - sensor fusion KW - multisensory perception KW - fusion KW - representation KW - multisensory processing KW - see KW - implementation KW - perspective KW - multisensory integration KW - population codes Y1 - 2013 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-122253 VL - 7 IS - 148 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Meule, Adrian T1 - Are certain foods addictive? JF - Frontiers in Psychiatry N2 - A commentary on Lifestyle medicine: the importance of firmgrounding on evidence by Rippe JM. Am J Lifestyle Med (2014) doi:10.1177/ 1559827613520527 Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-120257 SN - 1664-0640 VL - 5 IS - 38 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Sollfrank, Teresa A1 - Hart, Daniel A1 - Goodsell, Rachel A1 - Foster, Jonathan A1 - Tan, Tele T1 - 3D visualization of movements can amplify motor cortex activation during subsequent motor imagery JF - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience N2 - A repetitive movement practice by motor imagery (MI) can influence motor cortical excitability in the electroencephalogram (EEG). This study investigated if a realistic visualization in 3D of upper and lower limb movements can amplify motor related potentials during subsequent MI. We hypothesized that a richer sensory visualization might be more effective during instrumental conditioning, resulting in a more pronounced event related desynchronization (ERD) of the upper alpha band (10–12 Hz) over the sensorimotor cortices thereby potentially improving MI based brain-computer interface (BCI) protocols for motor rehabilitation. The results show a strong increase of the characteristic patterns of ERD of the upper alpha band components for left and right limb MI present over the sensorimotor areas in both visualization conditions. Overall, significant differences were observed as a function of visualization modality (VM; 2D vs. 3D). The largest upper alpha band power decrease was obtained during MI after a 3-dimensional visualization. In total in 12 out of 20 tasks the end-user of the 3D visualization group showed an enhanced upper alpha ERD relative to 2D VM group, with statistical significance in nine tasks.With a realistic visualization of the limb movements, we tried to increase motor cortex activation during subsequent MI. The feedback and the feedback environment should be inherently motivating and relevant for the learner and should have an appeal of novelty, real-world relevance or aesthetic value (Ryan and Deci, 2000; Merrill, 2007). Realistic visual feedback, consistent with the participant’s MI, might be helpful for accomplishing successful MI and the use of such feedback may assist in making BCI a more natural interface for MI based BCI rehabilitation. KW - 3-dimensional visualization KW - motor cortex activation KW - EEG KW - brain-computer interfaces Y1 - 2015 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-126058 VL - 9 IS - 463 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Körner, Anita A1 - Topolinski, Sascha A1 - Strack, Fritz T1 - Routes to Embodiment JF - Frontiers of Psychology N2 - Research on embodiment is rich in impressive demonstrations but somewhat poor in comprehensive explanations. Although some moderators and driving mechanisms have been identified, a comprehensive conceptual account of how bodily states or dynamics influence behavior is still missing. Here, we attempt to integrate current knowledge by describing three basic psychological mechanisms: direct state induction, which influences how humans feel or process information, unmediated by any other cognitive mechanism; modal priming, which changes the accessibility of concepts associated with a bodily state; sensorimotor simulation, which affects the ease with which congruent and incongruent actions are performed. We argue that the joint impact of these mechanisms can account for most existing embodiment effects. Additionally, we summarize empirical tests for distinguishing these mechanisms and suggest a guideline for future research about the mechanisms underlying embodiment effects. KW - priming KW - simulation KW - grounded cognition KW - embodied cognition KW - metaphors Y1 - 2015 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-125960 VL - 9 IS - 940 ER - TY - THES A1 - Holz, Elisa Mira T1 - Systematic evaluation of non-invasive brain-computer interfaces as assistive devices for persons with severe motor impairment based on a user-centred approach – in controlled settings and independent use T1 - Systematische Evaluation nicht-invasiver Gehirn-Computer Schnittstellen als Hilfsmittel für Personen mit schweren motorischen Einschränkungen auf Basis eines nutzerzentrierten Ansatzes – im kontrollierten Setting und experten-unabhängigen Gebrauch N2 - Brain-computer interfaces (BCIs) are devices that translate signals from the brain into control commands for applications. Within the last twenty years, BCI applications have been developed for communication, environmental control, entertainment, and substitution of motor functions. Since BCIs provide muscle independent communication and control of the environment by circumventing motor pathways, they are considered as assistive technologies for persons with neurological and neurodegenerative diseases leading to motor paralysis, such as amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS), muscular dystrophy, spinal muscular atrophy and stroke (Kübler, Kotchoubey, Kaiser, Wolpaw, & Birbaumer, 2001). Although most researcher mention persons with severe motor impairment as target group for their BCI systems, most studies include healthy participants and studies including potential BCI end-users are sparse. Thus, there is a substantial lack of studies that investigate whether results obtained in healthy participants can be transferred to patients with neurodegenerative diseases. This clearly shows that BCI research faces a translational gap between intense BCI research and bringing BCI applications to end-users outside the lab (Kübler, Mattia, Rupp, & Tangermann, 2013). Translational studies are needed that investigate whether BCIs can be successfully used by severely disabled end-users and whether those end-users would accept BCIs as assistive devices. Another obvious discrepancy exists between a plethora of short-term studies and a sparse number of long-term studies. BCI research thus also faces a reliability gap (Kübler, Mattia, et al., 2013). Most studies present only one BCI session, however the few studies that include several testing sessions indicate high inter- and intra-individual variance in the end-users’ performance due to non-stationarity of signals. Long-term studies, however, are needed to demonstrate whether a BCI can be reliably used as assistive device over a longer period of time in the daily-life of a person. Therefore there is also a great need for reliability studies. The purpose of the present thesis was to address these research gaps and to bring BCIs closer to end-users in need, especially into their daily-lives, following a user-centred design (UCD). The UCD was suggested as theoretical framework for bringing BCIs to end-users by Kübler and colleagues (Kübler et al., 2014; Zickler et al., 2011). This approach aims at the close and iterative interaction between BCI developers and end-users with the final goal to develop BCI systems that are accepted as assistive devices by end-users. The UCD focuses on usability, that is, how well a BCI technology matches the purpose and meets the needs and requirements of the targeted end-users and was standardized with the ISO 9241-210. Within the UCD framework, usability of a device can be defined with regard to its effectiveness, efficiency and satisfaction. These aspects were operationalized by Kübler and colleagues to evaluate BCI-controlled applications. As suggested by Vaughan and colleagues, the number of BCI sessions, the total usage duration and the impact of the BCI on the life of the person can be considered as indicators of usefulness of the BCI in long-term daily-life use (Vaughan, Sellers, & Wolpaw, 2012). These definitions and metrics for usability and usefulness were applied for evaluating BCI applications as assistive devices in controlled settings and independent use. Three different BCI applications were tested and evaluated by in total N=10 end-users: In study 1 a motor-imagery (MI) based BCI for gaming was tested by four end-users with severe motor impairment. In study 2, a hybrid P300 event-related (ERP) based BCI for communication was tested by four severely motor restricted end-users with severe motor impairment. Study 1 and 2 are short-term studies conducted in a controlled-setting. In study 3 a P300-ERP BCI for creative expression was installed for long-term independent use at the homes of two end-users in the locked-in state. Both end-users are artists who had gradually lost the ability to paint after being diagnosed with ALS. Results reveal that BCI controlled devices are accepted as assistive devices. Main obstacles for daily-life use were the not very aesthetic design of the EEG-cap and electrodes (cap is eye-catching and looks medical), low comfort (cables disturb, immobility, electrodes press against head if lying on a head cushion), complicated and time-consuming adjustment, low efficiency and low effectiveness, and not very high reliability (many influencing factors). While effectiveness and efficiency in the MI based BCI were lower compared to applications using the P300-ERP as input channel, the MI controlled gaming application was nevertheless better accepted by the end-users and end-users would rather like to use it compared to the communication applications. Thus, malfunctioning and errors, low speed, and the EEG cap are rather tolerated in gaming applications, compared to communication devices. Since communication is essential for daily-life, it has to be fast and reliable. BCIs for communication, at the current state of the art, are not considered competitive with other assistive devices, if other devices, such as eye-gaze, are still an option. However BCIs might be an option when controlling an application for entertainment in daily-life, if communication is still available. Results demonstrate that BCI is adopted in daily-life if it matches the end-users needs and requirements. Brain Painting serves as best representative, as it matches the artists’ need for creative expression. Caveats such as uncomfortable cap, dependence on others for set-up, and experienced low control are tolerated and do not prevent BCI use on a daily basis. Also end-users in real need of means for communication, such as persons in the locked-in state with unreliable eye-movement or no means for independent communication, do accept obstacles of the BCI, as it is the last or only solution to communicate or control devices. Thus, these aspects are “no real obstacles” but rather “challenges” that do not prevent end-users to use the BCI in their daily-lives. For instance, one end-user, who uses a BCI in her daily-life, stated: “I don’t care about aesthetic design of EEG cap and electrodes nor amplifier”. Thus, the question is not which system is superior to the other, but which system is best for an individual user with specific symptoms, needs, requirements, existing assistive solutions, support by caregivers/family etc.; it is thereby a question of indication. These factors seem to be better “predictors” for adoption of a BCI in daily-life, than common usability criterions such as effectiveness or efficiency. The face valid measures of daily-life demonstrate that BCI-controlled applications can be used in daily-life for more than 3 years, with high satisfaction for the end-users, without experts being present and despite a decrease in the amplitude of the P300 signal. Brain Painting re-enabled both artists to be creatively active in their home environment and thus improved their feelings of happiness, usefulness, self-esteem, well-being, and consequently quality of life and supports social inclusion. This thesis suggests that BCIs are valuable tools for people in the locked-in state. N2 - Gehirn-Computer Schnittstellen (engl. Brain-computer interfaces, Abk.: BCIs) sind technische Systeme, die Gehirnsignale in Kontrollbefehle für Computeranwendungen übersetzen. In den vergangenen zwanzig Jahren wurden verschiedenste BCI Anwendungen entwickelt, beispielsweise zur Kommunikation, Umweltsteuerung, Unterhaltung und Ersatz von Motorfunktionen. Da BCIs muskelunabhängige Kommunikation und Kontrolle ermöglichen, werden sie als mögliche Hilfsmittel für Personen mit neurologischen und neurodegenerativen Krankheiten, die zu motorischen Lähmungen führen, wie beispielsweise bei Amyotrophe Lateralsklerose (ALS), Muskeldystrophie, Spinale Muskelatrophie und Schlaganfall, in Betracht gezogen (Kübler, Kotchoubey, et al., 2001). Auch wenn die meisten BCI Forscher Personen mit starken motorischen Einschränkungen als Zielgruppe für ihre BCI Systeme angeben, so testen sie ihre Systeme nur in Stichproben von gesunden Probanden. BCI Studien, die Patienten einschließen, sind dagegen selten. Daher gibt es einen beträchtlichen Mangel an Studien, die untersuchen, ob die Forschungsergebnisse, die basierend auf einer gesunden Stichprobe verzeichnet wurden, auch auf Patienten mit neurodegenerativen Erkrankungen übertragen werden können. Das macht deutlich, dass es in der BCI Forschung eine erhebliche Translationslücke zwischen der intensiven BCI Grundlagenforschung und dem Transfer von BCI Anwendungen aus dem Labor zu den Patienten, den sogenannten BCI End-Nutzern, gibt (Kübler, Mattia, et al., 2013). Es werden deshalb Translationsstudien benötigt, die untersuchen, ob BCIs von stark motorisch eingeschränkten Patienten verwendet werden können und ob diese sogenannten End-Nutzer BCIs als Hilfsmittel akzeptieren. Zusätzlich ist eine deutliche Diskrepanz zwischen der Vielzahl an Kurzzeitstudien und der geringen Anzahl an Langzeitstudien zu verzeichnen. BCI Forschung ist daher auch mit einer Reliabilitätslücke konfrontiert (Kübler, Mattia, et al., 2013). Die meisten Studien basieren nur auf einer BCI Sitzung, jedoch zeigen die wenigen Studien, die auf mehreren Sitzungen beruhen, hohe inter- und intraindividuelle Varianz in der Performanz der Patienten. Langzeitzeitstudien werden daher benötigt, um aufzuzeigen, ob ein BCI reliabel als Hilfsmittel über einen längeren Zeitraum im Alltagsleben eines Patienten verwendet werden kann. Demzufolge gibt es einen starken Bedarf an Translations- und Reliabilitätsstudien. Das Anliegen der vorliegenden Dissertation war es, diesen Forschungslücken zu begegnen und, basierend auf einem nutzerzentrierten Vorgehen, BCIs näher zu den BCI End-Nutzern zu bringen, besonders in ihr Alltagsleben. Der nutzerzentrierte Ansatz wurde von Kübler und Kollegen (Kübler et al., 2014; Zickler et al., 2011) als theoretisches Gerüst nahegelegt, um BCIs näher zu Patienten zu bringen. Dieser Ansatz beabsichtigt eine enge und iterative Interaktion zwischen BCI Entwicklern und den End-Nutzern mit dem finalen Ziel BCI Systeme zu entwickeln, die von den End-Nutzern als Hilfsmittel akzeptiert werden. Der nutzerzentrierte Ansatz fokussiert auf die Benutzbarkeit, das heißt, wie gut eine BCI Technologie den Bedürfnissen und den Ansprüchen der Zielgruppe entspricht. Dieser Ansatz wurde standardisiert mit dem ISO 9241-210. Demnach ist die Benutzbarkeit eines Gerätes definiert hinsichtlich der Effektivität, Effizienz, und Zufriedenheit. Um BCI Systeme zu evaluieren, wurden diese Aspekte von Kübler und Kollegen operationalisiert. Nach Vaughan und Kollegen können die Anzahl der BCI Sitzungen, die Gesamtnutzungsdauer und der Einfluss eines BCIs auf die Lebensqualität einer Person als Indikatoren der Nützlichkeit eines BCI betrachtet werden (Vaughan et al., 2012). Diese Definitionen und entsprechenden Operationalisierungen wurden in dieser Dissertation verwendet, um BCI Anwendungen hinsichtlich ihrer Benutzbarkeit und Nützlichkeit als Hilfsmittel zu evaluieren. N=10 End-Nutzer testeten und evaluierten drei BCI Anwendungen: In Studie 1 wurde ein auf Bewegungsvorstellung basiertes BCI (engl. motor imagery, Abk: MI) zur Steuerung einer Spielanwendung von vier potentiellen End-Nutzern mit unterschiedlichen neurologischen Erkrankungen getestet. In Studie 2 wurde ein Hybrid BCI, das das P300 ereignis-korrelierte Potential (EKP) und Muskelaktivität als Inputkanäle zur Steuerung eines Kommunikationsprogramms verwendet, von vier potentiellen End-Nutzern getestet. Studie 1 und 2 sind Kurzzeitstudien, welche in einem kontrollierten Design durchgeführt wurden. In Studie 3 wurde ein P300-EKP basiertes BCI zum künstlerischen Ausdruck bei zwei End-Nutzern zuhause für einen experten-unabhängigen und längerfristigen Gebrauch implementiert. Beide End-Nutzer sind Künstler, die, aufgrund der Diagnose ALS und der damit verbundenen fast kompletten körperlichen Lähmung (Locked-in Zustand), nicht mehr in der Lage waren zu malen (Studie 3). Die Ergebnisse zeigen, dass BCI Systeme als Hilfsmittel akzeptiert werden. Das nicht sehr ästhetische Design der EEG-Kappe und der Elektroden (Kappe zu auffällig, sieht medizinisch aus), der geringe Komfort (Kabel stören, Immobilität, Elektroden drücken gegen den Kopf), die komplizierte und zeitaufwändige Einstellung und Anpassung, die geringe Effizienz und geringe Effektivität und die nicht sehr hohe Reliabilität (viele Einflussfaktoren), wurden jedoch für einen Alltagsgebrauch als problematisch angesehen. Obwohl die Effektivität und Effizienz beim MI BCI geringer, verglichen mit beiden P300-EKP BCI Systemen, waren, wurde das MI basierte BCI-Spiel von den End-Nutzern besser akzeptiert und die End-Nutzer konnten sich eher vorstellen es im Alltag zu verwenden, als das Kommunikationsprogramm. Das zeigt, dass Störungen und Fehler, eine geringe Geschwindigkeit, und die EEG Kappe bei BCI Systemen zur Unterhaltung eher toleriert werden, als bei Systemen zur Kommunikation. Da Kommunikation im Leben essentiell ist, muss sie schnell und zuverlässig sein. BCI Systeme zur Kommunikation sind daher zum aktuellen Stand der Technik nicht konkurrenzfähig mit anderen Hilfsmitteln, wenn andere Hilfsmittel zur Kommunikation, wie Augensteuerung, verwendet werden können. BCI Systeme sind aber eine Option im Bereich Unterhaltung, sofern Möglichkeiten zur Kommunikation (noch) bestehen. Die Ergebnisse zeigen, dass ein BCI für einen Alltagsgebrauch übernommen wird, wenn es den Bedürfnissen und Anforderungen des End-Nutzers entspricht. Brain Painting zeigt hierbei beispielhaft, wie negative Facetten, wie die wenig komfortable EEG Kappe, die Abhängigkeit von anderen aufgrund der komplexen Einstellung, und eine subjektiv empfundene geringe Kontrolle toleriert werden, da es genau den Bedürfnissen der Künstler sich kreativ auszudrücken entspricht. Ebenso Patienten, die Bedarf an Kommunikation haben, wie Patienten im Locked-in Zustand, die keine zuverlässigen Augen-Bewegungen aufweisen, oder Patienten, die keine Hilfsmittel zur unabhängigen Kommunikation haben, akzeptieren diese Umstände beim BCI Gebrauch. Das zeigt, dass diese Umstände keine richtigen „Hindernisse“, sondern vielmehr Herausforderungen sind, die eine Übernahme eines BCI im Alltag eines Patienten nicht verhindern. Es ist daher nicht die Frage, welches BCI System überlegen ist, sondern welches BCI System das Beste für ein Individuum mit spezifischen Symptomen, Bedürfnissen, Ansprüchen, vorhandenen Hilfsmitteln, Unterstützung durch Familie und Pflegern, ist; es ist deshalb eine Frage der Indikation. Eine End-Nutzerin, die ein BCI im Alltag verwendet, sagte beispielsweise: „Mir ist das ästhetische Design der EEG-Kappe und der Elektroden, oder des EEG-Verstärkers völlig egal“. Diese Faktoren können als die besten “Prädiktoren” für eine Übernahme eines BCI Systems im Alltag eines Patienten angesehen werden, weniger hingegen die üblichen Kriterien zur Bewertung der Benutzbarkeit, wie Effektivität und Effizienz. Die Ergebnisse hinsichtlich des Alltagsgebrauches belegen ferner, dass ein P300-EKP basiertes BCI mit hoher Zufriedenheit über einen Zeitraum von 3 Jahren, ohne die Hilfe eines BCI Experten, und trotz einer Abnahme der Amplitude des P300-Signales verwendet werden kann. Brain Painting ermöglichte beiden Künstlern sich wieder kreativ auszudrücken und beeinflusste somit positiv das Empfinden von Freude, das Gefühl von Nützlichkeit, das Selbstwertgefühl, das Wohlbefinden und folglich die Lebensqualität der Künstler und förderte ihre soziale Inklusion. Die vorliegende Dissertation zeigt, dass BCIs wertvolle Hilfsmittel für Personen im Locked-in Zustand sein können. KW - Gehirn-Computer-Schnittstelle KW - Benutzerfreundlichkeit KW - brain-computer interface KW - evaluation KW - Evaluation KW - usability KW - Benutzerfreundlichkeit/ Benutzbarkeit KW - assistive device KW - Hilfsmittel KW - user-centred design KW - nutzerzentrierter Ansatz Y1 - 2015 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-126334 ER - TY - THES A1 - Reicherts, Philipp T1 - Cognitive and Emotional Influences on Placebo Analgesia and Nocebo Hyperalgesia T1 - Kognititve und emotionale Einflussfaktoren auf Placeboanalgesie und Nocebohyperalgesie N2 - The perception of pain can be modulated by a variety of factors such as biological/pharmacological treatments as well as potent cognitive and emotional manipulations. Placebo and nocebo effects are among the most prominent examples for such manipulations. Placebo and nocebo manipulations cause reliable psychological and physiological changes, although the administered agent or treatment is inert. The present dissertation aimed at investigating the role of cognitive and emotional influences in the generation of placebo and nocebo effects on pain perception. In addition, the feasibility of solely psychological placebo manipulations to alter the perception of pain was tested. Two commonly discussed preconditions for the generation of placebo and nocebo effects are prior experiences (i.e., past encounter of drug effects) and expectations (i.e., positive or negative attitudes towards an intervention). So far, research on placebo and nocebo effects relied on the administration of sham interventions, which resembled medical treatments like inert pills, creams or injections. However, such experimental procedures deal with confounds due to earlier experiences and expectations resulting from the individual’s history with medical interventions. Accordingly, the implementation of a placebo manipulation that is completely new to an individual, seems necessary to disentangle the contribution of experience and expectation for the induction of placebo and nocebo effects. To this end, in Experiment 1 the level of experience and expectation regarding a placebo-nocebo treatment was stepwise manipulated across three different experimental groups. To avoid any resemblances to earlier experiences and individual expectations, a mere psychological placebo-nocebo treatment was chosen that was new to all participants. They were instructed that visual black and white stripe patterns had been found to reliably alter the perception of pain. One group of participants received only the placebo-nocebo instruction (expectation), a second group experienced a placebo-nocebo treatment within a conditioning phase (experience) but no instruction, and a third group received the combination of both that is a placebo-nocebo instruction and a placebo-nocebo conditioning (experience + expectation). It was shown that only the experience + expectation group revealed significantly higher pain ratings and physiological responses during nocebo, compared to placebo trials of the succeeding test phase. These findings demonstrate that the induction of a mere psychological placebo-nocebo effect on pain is in principle possible. Most important, results indicate that such effects most likely rely on both, a positive treatment experience, due to the encounter of an effective intervention (placebo conditioning), and a positive expectation about the intervention (placebo instruction).Besides experience and expectation, the current mood state has been shown to modulate pain and to impact the induction of placebo and nocebo effects. In this vein it has been demonstrated that placebo effects come along with positive affect, while nocebo effects often occur together with elevated feelings of anxiety. To clarify the interaction of emotions and placebo-nocebo manipulations on pain perception, in Experiment 2 the paradigm of Experiment 1 was modified. Instead of black and white stripe patterns, positive and negative emotional pictures were presented, which either cued pain increase (nocebo) or pain decrease (placebo). Two experimental groups were compared, which differed with regard to the instructed contingency of positive pictures serving as placebo and negative pictures serving as nocebo cues or vice versa (congruent vs. incongruent). Results indicate that the differentiation of placebo and nocebo trials (behaviorally and physiologically) was more pronounced for the congruent compared to the incongruent group. However, in the incongruent group, affective pain ratings were also significantly higher for nocebo (positive pictures) than placebo (negative pictures) trials, similar to the congruent group. These findings demonstrate that a placebo-nocebo manipulation is capable to dampen and even reverse the originally pain augmenting effect of negative emotions. The results of Experiment 2 were further corroborated in Experiment 3, when the design was adapted to the fMRI scanner, and again a congruent and an incongruent experimental group were compared. Behavioral, physiological and neurophysiological markers of pain processing revealed a differentiation between nocebo and placebo conditions that was present irrespective of the experimental group. In addition, the fMRI analysis revealed an increased engagement of prefrontal areas for the incongruent group only, supposedly reflecting the reinterpretation or appraisal process when positive pictures were cueing negative outcomes. Taken together, the results of the present studies showed (a) that it is possible to induce a placebo-nocebo effect on pain solely by a psychological manipulation, (b) that both, prior experiences and positive expectation, are necessary preconditions for this placebo-nocebo effect, (c) that the impact of negative emotion on pain can be dampened and even reversed by placebo-nocebo manipulations, and (d) that most likely a cognitive top-down process is crucial for the induction of (psychological) placebo-nocebo effects. These results significantly enhance our understanding of psychological mechanisms involved in the induction of placebo-nocebo effects. Further, a fruitful foundation for future studies is provided, which will need to determine the contributions of primarily nocebo or placebo responses mediating the effects as demonstrated in the present studies. In a long-term perspective, the present findings may also help to exploit placebo effects and prevent from nocebo effect in clinical contexts by further elucidating crucial psychological factors that contribute to the placebo and nocebo response. N2 - Die Wahrnehmung von Schmerz kann durch eine Vielzahl von Faktoren beeinflusst werden, darunter biologische und pharmakologische Interventionen sowie potente kognitive und emotionale Manipulationen. Placebo- und Nocebo-effekte gehören mit zu den eindrucksvollsten Beispielen für die Wirksamkeit derartiger Manipulationen. Placebo- und Nocebo-Behandlungen können zu manifesten psychologischen und physiologischen Veränderungen führen, obwohl die verabreichten Substanzen frei von Wirkstoffen bzw. den angewandten Scheinbehandlungen keine Wirkung zugeschrieben wird. In der vorliegenden Dissertation wurden kognitive und emotionale Einflussfaktoren auf die Induktion von Placebo- und Nocebo-Effekten bei der Wahrnehmung von Schmerz untersucht. Darüber hinaus sollte die Möglichkeit zur Verwendung rein psychologischer Placebo-Nocebo Manipulationen für die Modulation von Schmerz getestet werden. Zwei zentrale Voraussetzungen für die Erzeugung von Placebo und Nocebo-Effekten sind vorherige Erfahrung (z.B. auf Grund früherer Erfahrungen mit einem Medikament) und Erwartung (z.B. eine positive oder negative persönliche Einstellung gegenüber einer Therapie). Bisher basierte die Forschung zu Placebo- und Nocebo-Effekten vornehmlich auf Ergebnissen von Untersuchungen die Schein-Behandlungen oder Leerpräparate einsetzten wie z.B. Tabletten, Cremes oder Injektionen, die herkömmlichen medizinischen Interventionen sehr ähnlich sind. Jedoch ergibt sich bei einem derartigen experimentellen Vorgehen stets das Problem einer Konfundierung der Ergebnisse durch den Einfluss früherer Erfahrungen oder der individuellen Erwartungshaltung an die Behandlung, die aus einer Vorgeschichte medizinischer Therapieerlebnissen herrührt. Daraus leitet sich die Notwendigkeit von anderweitigen, dem Probanden völlig unbekannten Placebo-Interventionen ab, um die jeweilige Beteiligung von Erwartungs- und Erfahrungsprozessen für die Induktion von Placebo- und Nocebo-Effekten bestimmen zu können. Zu diesem Zweck wurden in Experiment 1 Erwartung und Erfahrung in drei Experimentalgruppen stufenweise und unabhängig voneinander manipuliert. Um einer Ähnlichkeit zu früheren Behandlungs-Erfahrungen und dadurch abgeleiteten Erwartungen vorzubeugen, wurde ein rein psychologisches Placebo-Nocebo Verfahren herangezogen, das mit Sicherheit allen Teilnehmern unbekannt war. Sie wurden darüber informiert, dass die Betrachtung von schwarz-weißen Streifenmustern eine wissenschaftlich bestätigte Wirkung auf die Schmerzwahrnehmung hätte. Eine Gruppe der Teilnehmer erhielt lediglich eine Placebo-Nocebo Instruktion (Erwartung), eine zweite Gruppe erlebte tatsächlich die Kopplung von zwei verschiedenen Streifenmustern mit unterschiedlich starken Schmerzreizen während einer Konditionierungs-Phase (Erfahrung) bekam aber keine Instruktion und eine dritte Gruppe erhielt sowohl die Placebo-Nocebo Instruktion als auch die Placebo-Nocebo Konditionierung (Erfahrung + Erwartung). Es konnte gezeigt werden, dass während der anschließenden Testphase lediglich die kombinierte Erfahrung + Erwartung Gruppe signifikant unterschiedliche Schmerzratings und physiologische Reaktionen auf die Schmerzreize während der Placebo- im Vergleich zu den Nocebo-Durchgängen aufwies. Diese Ergebnisse belegen, dass die Induktion eines rein psychologischen Placebo-Nocebo Effektes auf die Schmerzwahrnehmung prinzipiell möglich ist. Besonders hervorzuheben ist dabei die Notwendigkeit beider Prozesse, nämlich einer tatsächlichen Erfahrung der Wirksamkeit der Therapie (Placebo-Nocebo Konditionierung) und einer positiven Erwartung hinsichtlich der Intervention (Placebo-Nocebo Instruktion). Neben Erfahrung und Erwartung, hat die momentane Stimmung entscheidenden Einfluss auf die die Induktion von Placebo- und Nocebo-Effekten einerseits, sowie generell auf die Wahrnehmung von Schmerz andererseits. In diesem Zusammenhang konnte gezeigt werden, dass Placebo-Effekte mit einer Verbesserung der Stimmung einhergehen, Nocebo-Effekte hingegen häufig von gesteigerter Angst begleitet sind. Um die Interaktion von Emotionen und Placebo-Nocebo Manipulationen zu eruieren, wurde das in Experiment 1 etablierte Paradigma angewendet und modifiziert. Anstelle von Streifenmustern, wurden positive und negative emotionale Bilder präsentiert, die entweder eine Schmerz-Verstärkung (Nocebo) oder eine Schmerz-Linderung (Placebo) anzeigten. Zwei Experimentalgruppen wurden miteinander verglichen, die sich hinsichtlich der Kontingenz von positiven Bildern als Placebo- und negativen Bildern als Nocebo-Indikator, bzw. umgekehrt, positiven Bildern als Nocebo- und negativen Bildern als Placebo-Indikator, unterschieden (kongruent vs. inkongruent). Es zeigte, dass die Unterscheidung (Schmerzratings und physiologische Reaktionen auf den Schmerzreiz) zwischen Placebo- und Nocebo-Durchgängen in der kongruenten Gruppe stärker ausgeprägt war als in der inkongruenten Gruppe. Allerdings waren die affektiven Schmerzratings der inkongruenten Gruppe ebenfalls in Nocebo-Durchgängen (positive Bilder) signifikant höher als in Placebo-Durchgängen (negative Bilder), ähnlich zur kongruenten Gruppe. Die Daten zeigen damit, dass eine Placebo-Nocebo Manipulation in der Lage ist, die genuin Schmerz verstärkende Wirkung negativer Emotionen abzuschwächen und sogar umzukehren. Die Befunde aus Experiment 2 konnten zusätzlich in Experiment 3 gestützt werden, welches das zuvor getestete Design ins fMRT überführte und gleichermaßen eine kongruente und eine inkongruente Experimentalgruppe miteinander verglich. Verhaltensmaße sowie physiologische und neurophysiologische Korrelate der Schmerzwahrnehmung ergaben eine eindeutige Differenzierung zwischen Placebo- und Nocebo-Durchgängen, unabhängig von der Experimentalgruppe. Darüber hinaus zeigte sich in der inkongruenten Bedingung eine verstärkte präfrontale Aktivierung für den Vergleich von Nocebo- und Placebo-Durchgängen, was potenziell auf einen zusätzlichen Re- Interpretations- oder Appraisal-Prozess zurückzuführen ist, der sich einstellt, wenn ein positives Bild eine negative Konsequenz vorhersagt. Zusammengefasst zeigen die vorliegenden Studien, dass es (a) möglich ist einen Placebo-Nocebo Effekt mit einer rein psychologischen Manipulation hervorzurufen, dass (b) im Fall rein psychologischer Placebo-Nocebo Manipulationen sowohl Erfahrung als auch positive Erwartung notwendig sind, dass (c) der Einfluss negativer Emotionen auf Schmerz mittels einer Placebo-Nocebo Manipulation reduziert und sogar umgekehrt werden kann und (d) höchstwahrscheinlich ein kognitiver (Neu-) Bewertungsprozess für die Induktion (psychologischer) Placebo-Nocebo Effekte essentiell ist. Die Ergebnisse tragen zum Verständnis der beteiligten psychologischen Prozesse bei der Induktion von Placebo-Nocebo Effekten erheblich bei. Darüber hinaus stellen die verwendeten Paradigmen eine vielseitige Ausgangsposition für zukünftige Studien dar, die klären müssen, ob für die gefunden Ergebnisse vornehmlich Placebo- oder Nocebo-Effekte verantwortlich sind. Perspektivisch könnten die vorliegenden Befunden helfen, die psychologischen Grundlagen der Placebo-Nocebo Antwort näher zu beleuchten und damit sogar im klinischen Kontext zum Ausschöpfen von Placebo- sowie zur Vorbeugung von Nocebo-Effekten beizutragen. KW - Placebo KW - Nocebo-Effekt KW - Psychologie KW - Placebo Hypolagesia KW - Nocebo hyperoalgesia KW - Emotional Pain Modulation Y1 - 2013 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-106455 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Simon, Nadine A1 - Käthner, Ivo A1 - Ruf, Carolin A. A1 - Pasqualotto, Emanuele A1 - Kübler, Andrea A1 - Halder, Sebastian T1 - An auditory multiclass brain-computer interface with natural stimuli: Usability evaluation with healthy participants and a motor impaired end user JF - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience N2 - Brain-computer interfaces (BCIs) can serve as muscle independent communication aids. Persons, who are unable to control their eye muscles (e.g., in the completely locked-in state) or have severe visual impairments for other reasons, need BCI systems that do not rely on the visual modality. For this reason, BCIs that employ auditory stimuli were suggested. In this study, a multiclass BCI spelling system was implemented that uses animal voices with directional cues to code rows and columns of a letter matrix. To reveal possible training effects with the system, 11 healthy participants performed spelling tasks on 2 consecutive days. In a second step, the system was tested by a participant with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) in two sessions. In the first session, healthy participants spelled with an average accuracy of 76% (3.29 bits/min) that increased to 90% (4.23 bits/min) on the second day. Spelling accuracy by the participant with ALS was 20% in the first and 47% in the second session. The results indicate a strong training effect for both the healthy participants and the participant with ALS. While healthy participants reached high accuracies in the first session and second session, accuracies for the participant with ALS were not sufficient for satisfactory communication in both sessions. More training sessions might be needed to improve spelling accuracies. The study demonstrated the feasibility of the auditory BCI with healthy users and stresses the importance of training with auditory multiclass BCIs, especially for potential end-users of BCI with disease. KW - P300 KW - EEG KW - auditory BCI KW - brain-computer interface KW - communication KW - ALS Y1 - 2015 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-126450 VL - 8 IS - 1039 ER - TY - THES A1 - Wiemer, Julian T1 - Maintaining factors of fear-relevant illusory correlations T1 - Aufrechterhaltende Faktoren von angstbezogenen Zusammenhangsverschätzungen N2 - Biased cognitive processes are very likely involved in the maintenance of fears and anxiety. One of such cognitive processes is the perceived relationship between fear-relevant stimuli and aversive consequences. If this relationship is perceived although objective contingencies have been random, it is called an (a posteriori) illusory correlation. If this relationship is overestimated before objective contingencies are experienced, it is called an (a priori) expectancy bias. Previous investigations showed that fear-relevant illusory correlations exist, but very few is known about how and why this cognitive bias develops. In the present dissertation thesis, a model is proposed based on a review of the literature on fear-relevant illusory correlations. This model describes how psychological factors might have an influence on fear and illusory correlations. Several critical implications of the model were tested in four experiments. Experiment 1 tested the hypothesis that people do not only overestimate the proportion of aversive consequences (startle sounds) following emotionally negative stimuli (pictures of mutilations) relative to neutral stimuli (pictures of household objects), but also following highly arousing positive stimuli (pictures of erotic scenes), because arousal might be an important determinant of illusory correlations. The result was a significant expectancy bias for negative stimuli and a much smaller expectancy bias for positive stimuli. Unexpectedly, expectancy bias was restricted to women. An a posteriori illusory correlation was not found overall, but only in those participants who perceived the aversive consequences following negative stimuli as particularly aversive. Experiment 2 tested the same hypothesis as experiment 1 using a paradigm that evoked distinct basic emotions (pictures inducing fear, anger, disgust or happiness). Only negative emotions resulted in illusory correlations with aversive outcomes (startle sounds), especially the emotions of fear and disgust. As in experiment 1, the extent of these illusory correlations was correlated with the perceived aversiveness of aversive outcomes. Moreover, only women overestimated the proportion of aversive outcomes during pictures that evoked fear, anger or disgust. Experiment 3 used functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) to measure biased brain activity in female spider phobics during an illusory correlation paradigm. Both spider phobics and healthy controls expected more aversive outcomes (painful electrical shocks) following pictures of spiders than following neutral control stimuli (pictures of mushrooms). Spider phobics but not healthy controls overestimated the proportion of aversive outcomes following pictures of spiders in a trial-by-trial memory task. This a posteriori illusory correlation was correlated with enhanced shock aversiveness and activity in primary sensory-motor cortex in phobic participants. Moreover, spider phobics’ brain activity in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex was elevated in response to spider images. This activity also predicted the extent of the illusory correlation, which supports the theory that executive and attentional resources play an important role in the maintenance of illusory correlations. Experiment 4 tested the hypothesis that the enhanced aversiveness of some outcomes would be sufficient to causally induce an illusory correlation. Neutral images (colored geometric figures) were paired with differently aversive outcomes (three startle sounds varying in intensity). Participants developed an illusory correlation between those images, which predicted the most aversive sound and this sound, which means that this association was overestimated relative to the other associations. The extent of the illusory correlation was positively correlated with participants’ self-reported anxiety. The results imply that the previously found relationship between illusory correlations and outcome aversiveness might reflect a causal impact of outcome aversiveness or salience on illusory correlations. In sum, the conducted experiments indicate that illusory correlations between fear-relevant stimuli and aversive consequences might persist – among other factors - because of an enhanced aversiveness or salience of aversive consequences following feared stimuli. This assumption is based on correlational findings, a neural measure of outcome perception and a causal influence of outcome aversiveness on illusory correlations. Implications of these findings were integrated into a model of fear-relevant illusory correlations and potential implications are discussed. Future investigations should further elucidate the role of executive functions and gender effects. Moreover, the trial-by-trial assessment of illusory correlations is recommended to increase reliability of the concept. From a clinical perspective, the down-regulation of aversive experiences and the allocation of attention to non-aversive experiences might help to cure anxiety and cognitive bias. N2 - Verzerrte kognitive Prozesse sind sehr wahrscheinlich an der Aufrechterhaltung von Furcht und Angst beteiligt. Ein solcher kognitiver Prozess ist der wahrgenommene Zusammenhang zwischen Reizen, vor denen bereits Angst besteht und unangenehmen Konsequenzen. Wenn so ein Zusammenhang wahrgenommen wird, obwohl die objektiven Kontingenzen zufällig sind, spricht man von einer illusorischen Korrelation (a posteriori). Wenn so ein Zusammenhang überschätzt wird, bevor objektive Kontingenzen erlebt werden, spricht man von einer Erwartungsverzerrung (a priori). Frühere Untersuchungen zeigten, dass angstrelevante illusorische Korrelationen existieren, aber bisher ist nur wenig darüber bekannt, wie und warum diese entstehen. In der vorliegenden Dissertation wird ein Modell vorgeschlagen, dass auf bisherigen Erkenntnissen zu angstrelevanten illusorischen Korrelationen beruht. Das Modell beschreibt, welche psychologischen Faktoren die Entstehung von Angst und illusorischen Korrelationen begünstigen könnten. Mehrere Implikationen dieses Modells wurden in vier Experimenten getestet. Experiment 1 überprüfte die Hypothese, dass Menschen nicht nur die Häufigkeit unangenehmer Konsequenzen (Schreckgeräusche) nach emotional negativen Reizen (Bilder von Verletzungen) überschätzen, verglichen mit neutralen Reizen (Bilder von Haushaltsgegenständen), sondern auch nach sehr aufregenden positiven Reizen (Bilder von erotischen Szenen), weil die allgemeine Erregung einen Einfluss auf illusorische Korrelationen haben sollte. Das Ergebnis war eine signifikante Erwartungsverzerrung bei negativen Reizen und eine sehr viel kleinere Erwartungsverzerrung bei positiven Reizen. Unerwarteter Weise waren Erwartungsverzerrungen auf Frauen beschränkt. Eine illusorische Korrelation (a posteriori) konnte im Allgemeinen nicht festgestellt werden, sondern lediglich bei solchen Probanden, die die unangenehmen Konsequenzen nach negativen Reizen als besonders unangenehm empfanden. Experiment 2 überprüfte die gleiche Hypothese wie Experiment 1 anhand einer Versuchsanordnung, die verschiedene Basisemotionen hervorrufen sollte (durch Bilder, die Angst, Ärger, Ekel oder Freude induzierten). Nur negative Emotionen führten zu illusorischen Korrelationen (a posteriori) mit unangenehmen Ereignissen (Schreckgeräusche), insbesondere die Emotionen Angst und Ekel. Wie auch in Experiment 1 korrelierte das Ausmaß der illusorischen Korrelation mit der wahrgenommenen Unangenehmheit der unangenehmen Ereignisse bei der entsprechenden Bildkategorie. Darüber hinaus überschätzten nur Frauen den Anteil negativer Ereignisse bei Bildern, die Angst, Ekel, oder Ärger hervorriefen. Experiment 3 verwendete funktionelle Magnetresonanztomografie (fMRT), um verzerrte Gehirnaktivität bei Spinnenphobikerinnen während eines Versuchs zu illusorischen Korrelationen zu messen. Sowohl Spinnenphobikerinnen als auch gesunde Kontrollprobandinnen erwarteten mehr unangenehme Konsequenzen (schmerzhafte elektrische Reize) bei Bildern von Spinnen als bei neutralen Kontrollreizen (Bilder von Pilzen). Spinnenphobikerinnen, aber nicht gesunde Kontrollprobandinnen überschätzten jedoch im Nachhinein den Anteil unangenehmer Konsequenzen bei Bildern von Spinnen in einer Trial-by-Trial Gedächtnisaufgabe. Diese illusorische Korrelation (a posteriori) korrelierte mit der erhöhten Unangenehmheit der elektrischen Reize und mit Aktivierung im primären senso-motorischen Kortex der phobischen Teilnehmerinnen. Darüber hinaus wiesen Spinnenphobikerinnen in Reaktion auf die Bilder von Spinnen eine erhöhte Aktivierung im linken dorsolateralen präfrontalen Kortex auf. Diese Aktivität sagte auch das Ausmaß der illusorischen Korrelation vorher, was die These unterstützt, dass exekutive und Aufmerksamkeitsressourcen eine wichtige Rolle in der Aufrechterhaltung illusorischer Korrelationen spielen. Experiment 4 überprüfte die Hypothese, dass die erhöhte Unangenehmheit mancher Konsequenzen hinreichend sein würde, um kausal eine illusorische Korrelation hervorzurufen. Neutrale Bilder (geometrische Figuren in drei verschiedenen Farben) wurden mit unterschiedlich unangenehmen Konsequenzen gepaart (Schreckgeräusche in drei verschiedenen Intensitäten). Bei den Probanden entwickelte sich eine illusorische Korrelation mit der Farbe, die das unangenehmste Geräusch voraussagte und diesem Geräusch, das heißt, der Zusammenhang wurde im Vergleich zu den anderen Zusammenhängen überschätzt. Das Ausmaß der illusorischen Korrelation korrelierte positiv mit der Ängstlichkeit der Teilnehmer. Die Ergebnisse legen nahe, dass der bisher gefundene Zusammenhang zwischen illusorischen Korrelationen und der Unangenehmheit der unangenehmen Konsequenzen auf einen kausalen Einfluss der Unangenehmheit oder Salienz der Konsequenzen auf illusorische Korrelationen zurückgehen könnte. Zusammengefasst zeigten die durchgeführten Experimente, dass illusorische Korrelationen zwischen angstrelevanten Reizen und unangenehmen Konsequenzen – neben anderen Einflussfaktoren – aufgrund einer erhöhten Unangenehmheit oder Salienz unangenehmer Konsequenzen bei gefürchteten Reizen bestehen könnten. Diese Annahme basiert auf korrelativen Ergebnissen, einem neuralen Maß der Konsequenzverarbeitung und dem gefundenen kausalen Einfluss der Unangenehmheit unangenehmer Konsequenzen auf illusorische Korrelationen. Implikationen dieser Befunde werden in ein Modell zu angstrelevanten illusorischen Korrelationen integriert und diskutiert. Zukünftige Studien sollten die Rolle exekutiver Funktionen und Geschlechtsunterschiede genauer untersuchen. Es empfiehlt sich dabei, illusorische Korrelationen Trial-by-Trial zu erfassen, um die Reliabilität des Konzepts zu erhöhen. Aus klinischer Sicht könnten die Beruhigung unangenehmer Erfahrungen und die Aufmerksamkeitsallokation auf nicht-unangenehme Erfahrungen helfen, Ängste und kognitive Verzerrungen zu vermindern. KW - Verzerrte Kognition KW - Angststörung KW - Verzerrte Kognition KW - Illusorische Korrelation KW - Gefahrenlernen KW - Funktionelle Kernspintomografie KW - cognitive bias KW - illusory correlation KW - threat learning KW - functional magnetic resonance imaging KW - Assoziatives Gedächtnis Y1 - 2015 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-116960 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Reuss, Heiko A1 - Pohl, Carsten A1 - Kiesel, Andrea A1 - Kunde, Wilfried T1 - Instructed illiteracy reveals expertise-effects on unconscious processing JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - We used a new methodological approach to investigate whether top-down influences like expertise determine the extent of unconscious processing. This approach does not rely on preexisting differences between experts and novices, but instructs essentially the same task in a way that either addresses a domain of expertise or not. Participants either were instructed to perform a lexical decision task (expert task) or to respond to a combination of single features of word and non-word stimuli (novel task). The stimuli and importantly also the mapping of responses to those stimuli, however, were exactly the same in both groups. We analyzed congruency effects of masked primes depending on the instructed task. Participants performing the expert task responded faster and less error prone when the prime was response congruent rather than incongruent. This effect was significantly reduced in the novel task, and even reversed when excluding identical prime-target pairs. This indicates that the primes in the novel task had an effect on a perceptual level, but were not able to impact on response activation. Overall, these results demonstrate an expertise-based top-down modulation of unconscious processing that cannot be explained by confounds that are otherwise inherent in comparisons between novices and experts. KW - top-down control KW - expertise KW - masked priming KW - unconscious processing KW - lexical decision task Y1 - 2015 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-125332 VL - 6 IS - 239 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Eder, Andreas B. A1 - Deutsch, Roland T1 - Watch the target! Effects in the affective misattribution procedure become weaker (but not eliminated) when participants are motivated to provide accurate responses to the target JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - Previous research showed that priming effects in the affective misattribution procedure (AMP) are unaffected by direct warnings to avoid an influence of the primes. The present research examined whether a priming influence is diminished by task procedures that encourage accurate judgments of the targets. Participants were motivated to categorize the affective meaning of nonsense targets accurately by being made to believe that a true word was presented in each trial and by providing feedback on (allegedly) incorrect responses. This condition produced robust priming effects. Priming was however reduced and less reliable relative to more typical AMP conditions in which participants guessed the meaning of openly presented nonsense targets. Affective judgments of nonsense targets were not affected by advance knowledge of the response mapping during the priming phase, which argues against a response-priming explanation of AMP effects. These findings show that affective primes influence evaluative judgments even in conditions in which the motivation to provide accurate responses is high and a priming of motor responses is not possible. Priming effects were however weaker with high accuracy motivation, suggesting that a focus on accurate judgments is an effective strategy to control for an unwanted priming influence in the AMP. KW - implicit attitude measurement KW - accuracy motivation KW - affect misattribution procedure KW - response priming Y1 - 2015 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-125982 VL - 6 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Kleih, Sonja C. A1 - Herweg, Andreas A1 - Kaufmann, Tobias A1 - Staiger-Sälzer, Pit A1 - Gerstner, Natascha A1 - Kübler, Andrea T1 - The WIN-speller: a new intuitive auditory brain-computer interface spelling application JF - Frontiers in Neuroscience N2 - The objective of this study was to test the usability of a new auditory Brain-Computer Interface (BCI) application for communication. We introduce a word based, intuitive auditory spelling paradigm the WIN-speller. In the WIN-speller letters are grouped by words, such as the word KLANG representing the letters A, G, K, L, and N. Thereby, the decoding step between perceiving a code and translating it to the stimuli it represents becomes superfluous. We tested 11 healthy volunteers and four end-users with motor impairment in the copy spelling mode. Spelling was successful with an average accuracy of 84% in the healthy sample. Three of the end-users communicated with average accuracies of 80% or higher while one user was not able to communicate reliably. Even though further evaluation is required, the WIN-speller represents a potential alternative for BCI based communication in end-users. KW - Brain-Computer Interface (BCI) KW - communication KW - P300 KW - motor-impaired end-user KW - auditory Y1 - 2015 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-125972 VL - 9 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Wieser, Matthias J. A1 - Moscovitch, David A. T1 - The effect of affective context on visuocortical processing of neutral faces in social anxiety - An ERP study JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - It has been demonstrated that verbal context information alters the neural processing of ambiguous faces such as faces with no apparent facial expression. In social anxiety, neutral faces may be implicitly threatening for socially anxious individuals due to their ambiguous nature, but even more so if these neutral faces are put in self-referential negative contexts. Therefore, we measured event-related brain potentials (ERPs) in response to neutral faces which were preceded by affective verbal information (negative, neutral, positive). Participants with low social anxiety (LSA; n = 23) and high social anxiety (HSA; n = 21) were asked to watch and rate valence and arousal of the respective faces while continuous EEG was recorded. ERP analysis revealed that HSA showed elevated P100 amplitudes in response to faces, but reduced structural encoding of faces as indexed by reduced N170 amplitudes. In general, affective context led to an enhanced early posterior negativity (EPN) for negative compared to neutral facial expressions. Moreover, HSA compared to LSA showed enhanced late positive potentials (LPP) to negatively contextualized faces, whereas in LSA this effect was found for faces in positive contexts. Also, HSA rated faces in negative contexts as more negative compared to LSA. These results point at enhanced vigilance for neutral faces regardless of context in HSA, while structural encoding seems to be diminished (avoidance). Interestingly, later components of sustained processing (LPP) indicate that LSA show enhanced visuocortical processing for faces in positive contexts (happy bias), whereas this seems to be the case for negatively contextualized faces in HSA (threat bias). Finally, our results add further new evidence that top-down information in interaction with individual anxiety levels can influence early-stage aspects of visual perception. KW - context effects KW - face processing KW - social anxiety KW - ERPs (Event-Related Potentials) KW - EEG/ERP Y1 - 2015 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-125148 VL - 6 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Stippekohl, Bastian A1 - Winkler, Markus H. A1 - Walter, Bertram A1 - Kagerer, Sabine A1 - Mucha, Ronald F. A1 - Pauli, Paul A1 - Vaitl, Dieter A1 - Stark, Rudolf T1 - Neural Responses to Smoking Stimuli Are Influenced by Smokers' Attitudes towards Their Own Smoking Behaviour JF - PLoS One N2 - An important feature of addiction is the high drug craving that may promote the continuation of consumption. Environmental stimuli classically conditioned to drug-intake have a strong motivational power for addicts and can elicit craving. However, addicts differ in the attitudes towards their own consumption behavior: some are content with drug taking (consonant users) whereas others are discontent (dissonant users). Such differences may be important for clinical practice because the experience of dissonance might enhance the likelihood to consider treatment. This fMRI study investigated in smokers whether these different attitudes influence subjective and neural responses to smoking stimuli. Based on self-characterization, smokers were divided into consonant and dissonant smokers. These two groups were presented smoking stimuli and neutral stimuli. Former studies have suggested differences in the impact of smoking stimuli depending on the temporal stage of the smoking ritual they are associated with. Therefore, we used stimuli associated with the beginning (BEGIN-smoking-stimuli) and stimuli associated with the terminal stage (END-smoking-stimuli) of the smoking ritual as distinct stimulus categories. Stimulus ratings did not differ between both groups. Brain data showed that BEGIN-smoking-stimuli led to enhanced mesolimbic responses (amygdala, hippocampus, insula) in dissonant compared to consonant smokers. In response to END-smoking-stimuli, dissonant smokers showed reduced mesocortical responses (orbitofrontal cortex, subcallosal cortex) compared to consonant smokers. These results suggest that smoking stimuli with a high incentive value (BEGIN-smoking-stimuli) are more appetitive for dissonant than consonant smokers at least on the neural level. To the contrary, smoking stimuli with low incentive value (END-smoking-stimuli) seem to be less appetitive for dissonant smokers than consonant smokers. These differences might be one reason why dissonant smokers experience difficulties in translating their attitudes into an actual behavior change. KW - smoking habits KW - addiction KW - amygdala KW - behavioral addiction KW - addicts KW - nicotine addiction KW - drug research and development KW - drug addiction Y1 - 2012 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-124393 VL - 7 IS - 11 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Sütterlin, Stefan A1 - Paap, Muirne C. S. A1 - Babic, Stana A1 - Kübler, Andrea A1 - Vögele, Claus T1 - Rumination and Age: Some Things Get Better JF - Journal of Aging Research N2 - Rumination has been defined as a mode of responding to distress that involves passively focusing one's attention on symptoms of distress without taking action. This dysfunctional response style intensifies depressed mood, impairs interpersonal problem solving, and leads to more pessimistic future perspectives and less social support. As most of these results were obtained from younger people, it remains unclear how age affects ruminative thinking. Three hundred members of the general public ranging in age from 15 to 87 years were asked about their ruminative styles using the Response Styles Questionnaire (RSQ), depression and satisfaction with life. A Mokken Scale analysis confirmed the two-factor structure of the RSQ with brooding and reflective pondering as subcomponents of rumination. Older participants (63 years and older) reported less ruminative thinking than other age groups. Life satisfaction was associated with brooding and highest for the earlier and latest life stages investigated in this study. Y1 - 2012 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-124356 VL - 2012 IS - 267327 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Reuss, Heiko A1 - Kiesel, Andrea A1 - Kunde, Wilfried A1 - Wühr, Peter T1 - A cue from the unconscious - masked symbols prompt spatial anticipation JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - Anticipating where an event will occur enables us to instantaneously respond to events that occur at the expected location. Here we investigated if such spatial anticipations can be triggered by symbolic information that participants cannot consciously see. In two experiments involving a Posner cueing task and a visual search task, a central cue informed participants about the likely location of the next target stimulus. In half of the trials, this cue was rendered invisible by pattern masking. In both experiments, visible cues led to cueing effects, that is, faster responses after valid compared to invalid cues. Importantly, even masked cues caused cueing effects, though to a lesser extent. Additionally, we analyzed effects on attention that persist from one trial to the subsequent trial. We found that spatial anticipations are able to interfere with newly formed spatial anticipations and influence orienting of attention in the subsequent trial. When the preceding cue was visible, the corresponding spatial anticipation persisted to an extent that prevented a noticeable effect of masked cues. The effects of visible cues were likewise modulated by previous spatial anticipations, but were strong enough to also exert an impact on attention themselves. Altogether, the results suggest that spatial anticipations can be formed on the basis of unconscious stimuli, but that interfering influences like still active spatial anticipations can suppress this effect. KW - masked priming KW - unconscious processing KW - anticipation KW - endogenous shifts of attention KW - spatial cueing Y1 - 2012 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-123971 VL - 3 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Meule, Adrian A1 - Kübler, Andrea T1 - The translation of substance dependence criteria to food-related behaviors: different views and interpretations. JF - Frontiers in psychiatry N2 - No abstract available. Y1 - 2012 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-123092 ER - TY - THES A1 - Campbell, Marlen Jamie-Lee T1 - Organizational cultures’ impact on employees’ corruption T1 - Der Einfluß von Organisationskultur auf die Korruption von Mitarbeitern N2 - Although many researchers refer to organizational culture as the key to explain employees' organizational corruption (= corruption on behalf of the organization), literature lacks systematic empirical evidence. Through a mixed-method approach this research tries to shed some first lights on this issue with the questions: what characteristics describe an organizational culture that promotes employees' corruption? Does a certain type of organizational culture shape a positive attitude towards organizational corruption? Does organizational culture differ in its impact on different types of corruption? Does organizational culture interact with employees’ sex to promote employees’ corruption? And, is there a main effect of sex on corruption? A qualitative study investigates the characteristics of a corrupt organizational culture in both general and in particular for managers and employees (Study 1). 14 experts of different occupations were asked about underlying assumptions, values, and norms of a corrupt organizational culture coding the frequency and relationship of their answers. The results showed specific underlying assumptions, values, and norms that were shared by the interviewees and provide first insights into their interrelatedness. In addition, the quantitative field survey (Study 2) analyzed if a corrupt organizational culture shapes a positive attitude towards organizational corruption and if both tangible rewards and lax control mechanism mediate this impact. 131 participants answered questionnaires about their perceived competition in their industry, tangible rewards, lax control mechanism, and their attitude towards both gifting and bribery. Results showed that lax control mechanism (and for gifting also tangible rewards) mediated the positive impact of a corrupt organizational culture on organizational corruption. In addition, men and women did not differ in their attitude towards organizational corruption in a corrupt organizational culture. Finally a web-based experiment investigates if organizational culture shapes employees' corruption (Study 3). In addition this approach also covers if the impact of organizational culture on corruption depends on the type of corruption (organizational corruption vs. counterproductive), and if employees’ sex influence corruption and if there is an interaction of organizational culture and sex on employees’ corruption. 563 participants had to decide whether they engage in corruption. Although a corrupt organizational culture raises both types of corruption, there was neither a notable main effect of sex nor a high impact interaction effect of both on both types of corruption. Thus, aspects of a corrupt organizational culture seem to influence employees' corruption. N2 - Obwohl Organisationskultur von vielen Wissenschaftlern als eine Schlüsseldeterminante gesehen wird um organisationale Korruption (= Korruption im Sinne der Organisation) zu erklären, fehlt es in der fachwissenschaftlichen Literatur an systematischen empiristischen Belegen: Welche Eigenschaften zeichnen eine korruptionsfördernde Organisationskultur aus? Kann ein bestimmter Typ von Organisationskultur eine positive Einstellung gegenüber organisationaler Korruption formen? Und schließlich: unterscheiden sich die Einflussfaktoren für Korruption zwischen verschiedenen Korruptionstypen? Durch die Anwendung verschiedenere methodischer Zugänge versucht diese wissenschaftliche Arbeit erste Antworten in diesem Forschungsfeld zu geben. Gibt es eine Interaktion zwischen dem Geschlecht der Mitarbeiter und der Organisationskultur und unterscheiden sich Männer und Frauen in ihrem korrupten Verhalten? Zunächst werden anhand einer qualitativen Analyse von 14 Experteninterviews grundlegende Charakteristiken einer Organisationskultur in korrupten Organisationen analysiert (Studie 1). Die Experten aus verschiedenen Berufsfeldern wurden zu ihrer Wahrnehmung der Organisationskultur von verschiedenen korrupten Organisationen befragt. Die Ergebnisse zeigten, dass es spezifische unbewusste Annahmen, Werte und Normen gibt, die von den verschiedenen Experten berichtet worden sind. Diese Studie ermöglicht erste Einblicke in die Wirkung der und Verbindungen zwischen den von den Experten wahrgenommenen Normen, Werten und Annahmen. Weiterführend analysierte eine Feldstudie (Studie 2), ob eine korrupte Organisationskultur eine positive Wirkung auf die Einstellung von Mitarbeitern zu organisationale Korruption hat. Weiter wurde untersucht, ob konkrete antizipierte Belohnungen und laxe Kontrollmechanismen bei der Aufgabenerfüllungen den Effekt der korrupten Organisationskultur auf organisationale Korruption mediieren. 131 Probanden beantworteten verschiedene Fragebögen zu ihrer Wahrnehmung von Wettbewerb in ihrer Branche, laxe Kontrollmechanismen von Korruption, zu konkreten Belohnungen in ihrer Organisation und zu ihrer Einstellung zu Geschenke und Bestechung. Die Ergebnisse zeigen, dass es einen Haupteffekt von der korrupten Organisationskultur auf organisationale Korruption vorhanden ist, dass dieser durch konkrete Belohnungen und laxe Kontrollmechanismen (bei Geschenken) und durch laxe Kontrollmechanismen (bei Bestechung) mediiert wird. Männer und Frauen unterscheiden sich in einer korrupten Organisationskultur nicht hinsichtlich ihrer Einstellung zur Korruption. Abschließend analysiert Studie 3 als web-basiertes Experiment, ob sich die Organisationskultur auf Korruption auswirkt. 563 Probanden spielten einen männlichen oder weiblichen Angestellten, welcher entweder in einer ethischen oder korrupten Organisationskultur in einer fiktiven Organisation arbeitet. Alle Teilnehmer mussten sich in der Rolle entscheiden, ob er oder sie in verschiedenen Arten von Korruption (organisationale vs. kontraproduktive Korruption) aktiv werden will. Die Ergebnisse zeigen, dass eine korrupte Organisationskultur einen ähnlich positiven Einfluss auf beide Typen von Korruption hatte. Das Geschlecht hatte weder einen nennenswert signifikanten oder keinen Einfluss auf die Annahme der Korruption. Darüber hinaus gab es keine Interaktion zwischen Geschlecht und Organisationskultur für beide Typen der Korruption. KW - Korruption KW - Organisationskultur KW - Organisationspsychologie KW - organisationale Korruption KW - Korruption von Mitarbeitern KW - korrupte Organisationskultur KW - organizational corruption KW - employees' corruption KW - corrupt organizational culture KW - corruption KW - Organisation Y1 - 2015 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-123251 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Wölk, Julian A1 - Sütterlin, Stefan A1 - Koch, Stefan A1 - Vögele, Claus A1 - Schulz, Stefan M. T1 - Enhanced cardiac perception predicts impaired performance in the Iowa Gambling Task in patients with panic disorder JF - Brain and Behavior N2 - OBJECTIVE: Somatic marker theory predicts that somatic cues serve intuitive decision making; however, cardiovascular symptoms are threat cues for patients with panic disorder (PD). Therefore, enhanced cardiac perception may aid intuitive decision making only in healthy individuals, but impair intuitive decision making in PD patients. METHODS: PD patients and age-and sex-matched volunteers without a psychiatric diagnosis (n=17, respectively) completed the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) as a measure of intuitive decision making. Interindividual differences in cardiac perception were assessed with a common mental-tracking task. RESULTS: In line with our hypothesis, we found a pattern of opposing associations (Fisher's Z=1.78, P=0.04) of high cardiac perception with improved IGT-performance in matched control-participants (r=0.36, n=14) but impaired IGT-performance in PD patients (r=-0.38, n=13). CONCLUSION: Interoceptive skills, typically assumed to aid intuitive decision making, can have the opposite effect in PD patients who experience interoceptive cues as threatening, and tend to avoid them. This may explain why PD patients frequently have problems with decision making in everyday life. Screening of cardiac perception may help identifying patients who benefit from specifically tailored interventions. KW - panic disorder KW - somatic marker hypothesis KW - cardiac perception KW - Iowa Gambling Task KW - decision making KW - interoception Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-119865 SN - 2162-3279 VL - 4 IS - 2 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Meule, Adrian A1 - Lutz, Annika P. C. A1 - Krawietz, Vera A1 - Stützer, Judith A1 - Vögele, Claus A1 - Kübler, Andrea T1 - Food-cue affected motor response inhibition and self-reported dieting success: a pictorial affective shifting task JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - Behavioral inhibition is one of the basic facets of executive functioning and is closely related to self-regulation. Impulsive reactions, that is, low inhibitory control, have been associated with higher body mass index (BMI), binge eating, and other problem behaviors (e.g., substance abuse, pathological gambling, etc.). Nevertheless, studies which investigated the direct influence of food-cues on behavioral inhibition have been fairly inconsistent. In the current studies, we investigated food-cue affected behavioral inhibition in young women. For this purpose, we used a go/no-go task with pictorial food and neutral stimuli in which stimulus-response mapping is reversed after every other block (affective shifting task). In study 1, hungry participants showed faster reaction times to and omitted fewer food than neutral targets. Low dieting success and higher BMI were associated with behavioral disinhibition in food relative to neutral blocks. In study 2, both hungry and satiated individuals were investigated. Satiation did not influence overall task performance, but modulated associations of task performance with dieting success and self-reported impulsivity. When satiated, increased food craving during the task was associated with low dieting success, possibly indicating a preload-disinhibition effect following food intake. Food-cues elicited automatic action and approach tendencies regardless of dieting success, self-reported impulsivity, or current hunger levels. Yet, associations between dieting success, impulsivity, and behavioral food-cue responses were modulated by hunger and satiation. Future research investigating clinical samples and including other salient non-food stimuli as control category is warranted. KW - impulsivity KW - inhibitory control KW - response inhibition KW - go/no-go task KW - food-cues KW - dieting success KW - body mass index Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-119840 SN - 1664-1078 VL - 5 ER - TY - THES A1 - Erlbeck, Helena T1 - The event-related potentials Mismatch Negativity, P300, and N400: Effects of attentional modulation and application in patients with disorders of consciousness T1 - Die ereigniskorrelierten Potentiale Mismatch Negativity, P300, und N400: Effekte von Aufmerksamkeitsmodulation und Anwendung in Patienten mit Störungen des Bewusstseins N2 - The present work comprises four studies dealing with the investigation of the auditory event-related potentials (ERP) Mismatch Negativity (MMN), P300, and N400 under different attentional instructions, and with their application in patients with disorders of consciousness (DOC) to assess residual cognitive functioning. In guided interviews (study 1), practitioners working with DOC patients stated their general interest in and an objective need for the complementation of current diagnostic procedures by reliable and valid ERP-based methods. Subsequently, in study 2, simple oddball and semantic paradigms were applied to 19 behaviorally non-responsive DOC patients revealing the presence of at least one ERP in eight patients investigated. In the third and fourth study, specific attentional effects on ERPs were investigated in healthy participants to define optimal instructions and stimulus parameters. In study 3, MMN and N400 amplitudes were assessed in 18 participants, and in study 4, MMN and P300 amplitudes were assessed in 32 participants. Both studies included an ignore task (attention on simultaneous visual stimuli), a passive task, and a focused task and revealed distinct attentional effects on P300 and N400 with largest amplitudes in the focused task, smaller ones in the passive task and no ERP in the ignore task. An MMN was elicited in all tasks, but still, amplitudes differed as a function of task. In addition, study 4 included oddball paradigms comprising several deviants in different dimensions. Higher amplitudes were found in this multifeature paradigm compared to traditional oddball paradigms and larger amplitudes were elicited by deviants highly different from standards. It is concluded that ERPs represent a promising tool to complement clinical assessment of DOC patients. Application of ERP paradigms should include focused instructions, especially when using semantic material. Furthermore, multifeature paradigms have been proven especially useful eliciting large amplitudes and allowing for the investigation of several dimensions of deviants at the same time. N2 - Die vorliegende Arbeit beinhaltet vier Studien, die die auditorischen ereigniskorrelierten Potentiale (EKP) Mismatch Negativität (MMN), P300, und N400 unter verschiedenen Instruktionen untersuchen, und deren Anwendung bei Patienten mit Bewusstseinsstörungen darstellen. In Studie 1 äußerten neurologische Fachärzte in Leitfadeninterviews ein generelles Interesse und eine objektive Notwendigkeit der Ergänzung bisheriger diagnostischer Vorgehensweisen durch EKP-basierte Methoden. In Studie 2 wurden 19 motorisch nicht-responsiven Patienten verschiedene Stimuli in Form einfacher Oddball-Paradigmen und semantischen Materials dargeboten und es konnte in acht Patienten mindestens ein EKP nachgewiesen wer-den. Studie 3 und 4 dienten der Untersuchung spezifischer Aufmerksamkeitseffekte auf EKPs in Gesunden, um optimale Instruktionen und Stimulusparameter zu definieren. Es wurden jeweils MMN und N400 in 18 Teilnehmern und MMN und P300 in 32 Teilnehmern untersucht. Beide Studien enthielten eine Ablenkungsaufgabe (simultane visuelle Reize), eine passive und eine fokussierte Aufgabe und zeigten deutliche Aufgabeneffekte auf P300 und N400. Die höchsten Amplituden wurden in der fokussierten Aufgabe ausgelöst, kleinere in der passiven und kein EKP in der Ablenkungsaufgabe. Eine MMN wurde in allen Aufgaben ausgelöst, aber auch hier unterschieden sich die Amplituden in Abhängigkeit der Aufgabe. Studie 4 ent-hielt außerdem ein Oddball mit mehreren abweichenden Tönen in vier Dimensionen. Dieses erzielte höhere Amplituden als das klassische Oddball mit nur einem abweichenden Ton. Hö-here Amplituden wurden von abweichenden Tönen ausgelöst, welche sich stark vom Standardton unterschieden. EKPs stellen ein vielversprechendes Instrument zur Ergänzung klini-scher Diagnosen bewusstseinsgestörter Patienten dar. Es sollte auf eindeutig zu differenzierende abweichende Reize und bei semantischen Material auf fokussierte Instruktionen zurückgegriffen werden. Paradigmen mit verschiedenen abweichenden Tönen können aufgrund höherer Amplituden und eines umfassenden Reizverarbeitungsprofils besonders nützlich sein. KW - event-related potentials KW - attention KW - ereigniskorrelierte Potentiale KW - Aufmerksamkeit KW - disorders of consciousness KW - Bewusstseinsstörungen KW - Bewusstseinsstörung KW - Ereigniskorreliertes Potenzial KW - Bewusstsein Y1 - 2015 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-121041 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Höhne, Johannes A1 - Holz, Elisa A1 - Staiger-Sälzer, Pit A1 - Müller, Klaus-Robert A1 - Kübler, Andrea A1 - Tangermann, Michael T1 - Motor Imagery for Severely Motor-Impaired Patients: Evidence for Brain-Computer Interfacing as Superior Control Solution JF - PLoS ONE N2 - Brain-Computer Interfaces (BCIs) strive to decode brain signals into control commands for severely handicapped people with no means of muscular control. These potential users of noninvasive BCIs display a large range of physical and mental conditions. Prior studies have shown the general applicability of BCI with patients, with the conflict of either using many training sessions or studying only moderately restricted patients. We present a BCI system designed to establish external control for severely motor-impaired patients within a very short time. Within only six experimental sessions, three out of four patients were able to gain significant control over the BCI, which was based on motor imagery or attempted execution. For the most affected patient, we found evidence that the BCI could outperform the best assistive technology (AT) of the patient in terms of control accuracy, reaction time and information transfer rate. We credit this success to the applied user-centered design approach and to a highly flexible technical setup. State-of-the art machine learning methods allowed the exploitation and combination of multiple relevant features contained in the EEG, which rapidly enabled the patients to gain substantial BCI control. Thus, we could show the feasibility of a flexible and tailorable BCI application in severely disabled users. This can be considered a significant success for two reasons: Firstly, the results were obtained within a short period of time, matching the tight clinical requirements. Secondly, the participating patients showed, compared to most other studies, very severe communication deficits. They were dependent on everyday use of AT and two patients were in a locked-in state. For the most affected patient a reliable communication was rarely possible with existing AT. KW - eyes KW - signal filtering KW - social communication KW - hands KW - machine learning KW - man-computer interface KW - games KW - electroencephalography Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-119331 VL - 9 IS - 8 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Meule, Adrian A1 - Gearhard, Ashley N. T1 - Food Addiction in the Light of DSM-5 JF - Nutrients N2 - The idea that specific kind of foods may have an addiction potential and that some forms of overeating may represent an addicted behavior has been discussed for decades. In recent years, the interest in food addiction is growing and research on this topic lead to more precise definitions and assessment methods. For example, the Yale Food Addiction Scale has been developed for the measurement of addiction-like eating behavior based on the diagnostic criteria for substance dependence of the fourth revision of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV). In 2013, diagnostic criteria for substance abuse and-dependence were merged, thereby increasing the number of symptoms for substance use disorders (SUDs) in the DSM-5. Moreover, gambling disorder is now included along SUDs as a behavioral addiction. Although a plethora of review articles exist that discuss the applicability of the DSM-IV substance dependence criteria to eating behavior, the transferability of the newly added criteria to eating is unknown. Thus, the current article discusses if and how these new criteria may be translated to overeating. Furthermore, it is examined if the new SUD criteria will impact future research on food addiction, for example, if "diagnosing" food addiction should also be adapted by considering all of the new symptoms. Given the critical response to the revisions in DSM-5, we also discuss if the recent approach of Research Domain Criteria can be helpful in evaluating the concept of food addiction. KW - food addiction KW - obesity KW - binge eating KW - RDoC KW - DSM-IV KW - DSM-5 KW - substance dependence KW - substance use disorder KW - gambling KW - craving Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-119279 SN - 2072-6643 VL - 6 IS - 9 ER - TY - THES A1 - Zürn, Michael T1 - The Dual Nature of Utility - Categorical and Comparative Evaluations in Economic Decisions T1 - Die Dualität des Nutzens - kategoriale und komparative Bewertungen in ökonomischen Entscheidungen N2 - Utility is perhaps the most central concept in modern economic theorizing. However, the behaviorist reduction to Revealed Preference not only removed the psychological content of utility but experimental investigations also exposed numerous anomalies in this theory. This program of research focused on the psychological processes by which utility judgments are generated. For this purpose, the standard assumption of a homogeneous concept is substituted by the Utilitarian Duality Hypothesis. In particular, judgments concerning categorical utility (uCat) infer an object's category based on its attributes which may subsequently allow the transfer of evaluative information like feelings or attitudes. In contrast, comparative utility (uCom) depends on the distance to a reference value on a specific dimension of comparison. Importantly, dimensions of comparison are manifold and context dependent. In a series of experiments, we show that the resulting Dual Utility Model is able to explain several known anomalies in a parsimonious fashion. Moreover, we identify central factors determining the relative weight assigned to both utility components. Finally, we discuss the implications of the Utilitarian Duality for both, the experimental practice in economics as well as the consequences for economic theorizing. In sum, we propose that the Dual Utility Model can serve as an integrative framework for both the rational model and its anomalies. N2 - Der Nutzen ist wohl eines der meist beachteten Konzepte der ökonomischen Theorie. Allerdings entfernte die behavioristische Reduktion auf offenbarte Präferenzen nicht nur den psychologischen Inhalt des Nutzens, sondern zeigte darüber hinaus in experimentellen Untersuchungen auch zahlreiche Anomalien der Theorie auf. Das vorliegende Forschungsprogramm stellt die psychologischen Prozesse in den Vordergrund, mittels derer Nutzen beurteilt wird. Zu diesem Zweck wird die verbreitete Annahme eines homogenen Konzepts durch die Zwei-Nutzen-Hypothese ersetzt. Im Besonderen bestimmen Urteile über den kategorialen Nutzen (uCat) anhand der Attribute eines Objekts zunächst dessen Kategorie, wodurch daraufhin bewertende Informationen, wie z.B. Gefühle oder Einstellungen, auf das Objekt übertragen werden können. Demgegenüber bestimmt sich der komparative Nutzen (uCom) über die Abweichung von einem Referenzwert in einer bestimmten Vergleichsdimension, welche generell zahlreich und kontextabhängig sein können. In einer Serie von Experimenten wird gezeigt, dass das resultierende Zwei-Nutzen-Modell eine Reihe von bekannten Anomalien in einer sparsamen Weise erklären kann. Darüber hinaus werden zentrale Faktoren identifiziert, welche die relative Gewichtung beider Nutzenkomponenten bestimmen. Schließlich werden die Implikationen der Zwei-Nutzen-Hypothese für die experimentelle Praxis und die ökonomische Theorie diskutiert. Zusammenfassend wird ausgeführt, dass das entwickelte Zwei-Nutzen-Modell als integrativer Rahmen für sowohl das rationale Modell als auch für seine Anomalien dienen kann. KW - Nutzen KW - Psychologie KW - Ultimatum Game KW - transaction utility KW - preference construction KW - utility KW - revealed preference KW - Wert KW - Preisvergleich KW - Nutzenvergleich Y1 - 2015 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-120141 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Wieser, Matthias J. A1 - Gerdes, Antje B. M. A1 - Reicherts, Philipp A1 - Pauli, Paul T1 - Mutual influences of pain and emotional face processing JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - The perception of unpleasant stimuli enhances whereas the perception of pleasant stimuli decreases pain perception. In contrast, the effects of pain on the processing of emotional stimuli are much less known. Especially given the recent interest in facial expressions of pain as a special category of emotional stimuli, a main topic in this research line is the mutual influence of pain and facial expression processing. Therefore, in this mini-review we selectively summarize research on the effects of emotional stimuli on pain, but more extensively turn to the opposite direction namely how pain influences concurrent processing of affective stimuli such as facial expressions. Based on the motivational priming theory one may hypothesize that the perception of pain enhances the processing of unpleasant stimuli and decreases the processing of pleasant stimuli. This review reveals that the literature is only partly consistent with this assumption: pain reduces the processing of pleasant pictures and happy facial expressions, but does not – or only partly – affect processing of unpleasant pictures. However, it was demonstrated that pain selectively enhances the processing of facial expressions if these are pain-related (i.e., facial expressions of pain). Extending a mere affective modulation theory, the latter results suggest pain-specific effects which may be explained by the perception-action model of empathy. Together, these results underscore the important mutual influence of pain and emotional face processing. KW - emotion KW - facial expression KW - ERPs KW - perception-action KW - pain Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-118446 SN - 1664-1078 VL - 5 ER - TY - THES A1 - Wu, Lingdan T1 - Emotion Regulation in Addicted Smokers T1 - Emotionsregulation bei abhängigen Rauchern N2 - Background: Nicotine addiction is the most prevalent type of drug addiction that has been described as a cycle of spiraling dysregulation of the brain reward systems. Imaging studies have shown that nicotine addiction is associated with abnormal function in prefrontal brain regions that are important for cognitive emotion regulation. It was assumed that addicts may perform less well than healthy nonsmokers in cognitive emotion regulation tasks. The primary aims of this thesis were to investigate emotional responses to natural rewards among smokers and nonsmokers and to determine whether smokers differ from nonsmokers in cognitive regulation of positive and negative emotions. To address these aims, two forms of appraisal paradigms (i.e., appraisal frame and reappraisal) were applied to compare changes in emotional responses of smokers with that of nonsmokers as a function of appraisal strategies. Experiment 1: The aim of the first experiment was to evaluate whether and how appraisal frames preceding positive and negative picture stimuli affect emotional experience and facial expression of individuals. Twenty participants were exposed to 125 pairs of auditory appraisal frames (either neutral or emotional) followed by picture stimuli reflecting five conditions: unpleasant-negative, unpleasant-neutral, pleasant-positive, pleasant-neutral and neutral-neutral. Ratings of valence and arousal as well as facial EMG activity over the corrugator supercilii and the zygomaticus major were measured simultaneously. The results indicated that appraisal frames could alter both subjective emotional experience and facial expressions, irrespective of the valence of the pictorial stimuli. These results suggest and support that appraisal frame is an efficient paradigm in regulation of multi-level emotional responses. 8 Experiment 2: The second experiment applied the appraisal frame paradigm to investigate how smokers differ from nonsmokers on cognitive emotion regulation. Sixty participants (22 nonsmokers, 19 nondeprived smokers and 19 12-h deprived smokers) completed emotion regulation tasks as described in Experiment 1 while emotional responses were concurrently recorded as reflected by self-ratings and psychophysiological measures (i.e., facial EMG and EEG). The results indicated that there was no group difference on emotional responses to natural rewards. Moreover, nondeprived smokers and deprived smokers performed as well as nonsmokers on the emotion regulation task. The lack of group differences in multiple emotional responses (i.e., self-reports, facial EMG activity and brain EEG activity) suggests that nicotine addicts have no deficit in cognitive emotion regulation of natural rewards via appraisal frames. Experiment 3: The third experiment aimed to further evaluate smokers’ emotion regulation ability by comparing performances of smokers and nonsmokers in a more challenging cognitive task (i.e., reappraisal task). Sixty-five participants (23 nonsmokers, 22 nondeprived smokers and 20 12-h deprived smokers) were instructed to regulate emotions by imagining that the depicted negative or positive scenario would become less negative or less positive over time, respectively. The results showed that nondeprived smokers and deprived smokers responded similarly to emotional pictures and performed as well as nonsmokers in down-regulating positive and negative emotions via the reappraisal strategy. These results indicated that nicotine addicts do not have deficit in emotion regulation using cognitive appraisal strategies. In sum, the three studies consistently revealed that addicted smokers were capable to regulate emotions via appraisal strategies. This thesis establishes the groundwork for therapeutic use of appraisal instructions to cope with potential self-regulation failures in nicotine addicts. N2 - Hintergrund: Nikotinsucht ist die am weitesten verbreitete Form von Drogenabhängigkeit und wird beschrieben als eine immer stärker werdende Dysregulation des Belohnungssystems im Gehirn. Bildgebende Studien zeigten, dass Nikotinabhängige eine abnormale Funktion der präfrontalen Gehirnregionen aufweisen, die für die kognitive Emotionsregulation von entscheidender Bedeutung sind. Es wurde angenommen, dass Süchtige bei kognitiven Aufgaben zur Emotionsregulation schlechter abschneiden als gesunde Nichtraucher. Vorrangige Ziele dieser Thesis waren die Untersuchung emotionaler Reaktionen auf natürliche, Raucher-irrelevante Stimuli bei Rauchern und Nichtrauchern. Außerdem sollte herausgefunden werden, ob sich Raucher von Nichtrauchern bezüglich ihrer kognitiven Regulation von positiven und negativen Emotionen unterscheiden. Um diese Veränderungen in der emotionalen Reaktion in Abhängigkeit der Interpretationsstrategie vergleichen zu können, wurden zwei Paradigmen zur Einschätzung emotionaler Stimuli eingesetzt: Eine prospektive Interpretationsstrategie des kommenden Stimulus (appraisal frame) und eine retrospektive Interpretationsstrategie nach der Stimuluspräsentation (reappraisal). Experiment 1: Ziel des ersten Experiments war die Evaluierung ob und wie Interpretationen vor positiven oder negativen Stimulusbildern die emotionale Erfahrung und den Gesichtsausdruck von Personen beeinflussen. 20 Versuchspersonen wurden 125 Paare auditiver Beschreibungen (entweder neutral oder emotional) präsentiert, gefolgt von Stimulusbildern, die zusammen fünf Stimulus-Kategorien bildeten: unangenehm – negativ, unangenehm – neutral, angenehm – positiv, angenehm – neutral und neutral – neutral. Valenz- und Arousal-Ratings wurden abgefragt und die EMG-Aktivität der Gesichtsmuskeln corrugator supercilii und zygomaticus 10 major wurden zeitgleich aufgenommen. Die Ergebnisse zeigten, dass appraisal frames sowohl emotionale Reaktionen einschließlich subjektiver emotionaler Erfahrungen beeinflussen als auch den Gesichtsausdruck verändern können, unabhängig von der Valenz des Bildstimulus. Dies zeigt und beweist die Effizienz des appraisal frame Paradigmas bei der Regulation von emotionalen Reaktionen auf mehreren Verarbeitungsebenen. Experiment 2: Das zweite Experiment bezog sich auf das appraisal frame Paradigma und sollte untersuchen wie sich Raucher von Nichtrauchern in ihrer kognitiven Emotionsregulation unterscheiden. 60 Probanden (22 Nichtraucher, 19 Raucher ohne Entzug und 19 Raucher mit 12 Stunden Zigarettenentzug) führten Emotionsregulationsaufgaben wie in Experiment 1 beschrieben aus, während ihre emotionalen Reaktionen ständig über Selbsteinschätzungen und psychophysiologische Messungen aufgenommen wurden (faziales EMG und EEG). Die Ergebnisse zeigten keine Gruppenunterschieden bei den emotionalen Reaktionen auf natürliche Stimuli, ohne Bezug zum Rauchen; Außerdem schnitten Raucher mit und ohne Zigarettenentzug in der Emotionsregulationsaufgabe genauso gut ab wie Nichtraucher. Die gleichen Ergebnisse in allen Gruppen hinsichtlich emotionaler Reaktionen (Selbsteinschätzung, faziale EMG Aktivität und EEG Aktivität) machten deutlich, dass Nikotinabhängige keine Einschränkungen in der kognitiven Emotionsregulation auf natürliche Stimuli mittels Vorbeurteilungen haben. Experiment 3: Der dritte Versuch wurde durchgeführt, um die Fähigkeiten von Rauchern bei der Emotionsregulation zu untersuchen, indem die Erfolge von Rauchern und Nichtrauchern in einer schwierigeren kognitiven Aufgabe (reappraisal task) verglichen wurden. 65 Versuchspersonen (23 Nichtraucher, 22 Raucher ohne Entzug und 20 Raucher mit 12 Stunden Zigarettenentzug) wurden instruiert ihre Emotionen zu regulieren, indem sie emotionale Bilder 11 mit neutralem Gefühl interpretieren. Die Probanden sollten sich vorstellen, dass die negativen oder positiven Syenarios immer weniger negativ oder weniger positiv werden. Die Ergebnisse stellen heraus, dass Raucher mit und ohne Zigarettenentzug ähnlich auf emotionale Bilder reagierten und ihre positiven und negativen Emotionen mit der reappraisal Strategie genauso gut herunterregulierten wie Nichtraucher. Zusammenfassend machen die drei Studien deutlich, dass Nikotinabhängige mittels Interpretationsstrategien ihre Emotionen regulieren können. Diese Thesis bilden das Fundament für den therapeutischen Nutzen von Interpretationsstrategien, damit Nikotinabhängige mit potenziellen Selbstregulationsstörungen umgehen können. KW - Gefühl KW - Regulation KW - Rauch KW - Elektroencephalogramm KW - Elektromyographie KW - Emotion KW - Regulation KW - Smoke KW - Electroencephalography KW - Electromyography Y1 - 2013 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-85471 ER - TY - THES A1 - Bakhtiari, Giti T1 - The Role of Fluency in Oral Approach and Avoidance T1 - Die Rolle von Verarbeitungsflüssigkeit in oraler Annäherung und Vermeidung N2 - Names of, for instance, children or companies are often chosen very carefully. They should sound and feel good. Therefore, many companies try to choose artificially created names that can easily be pronounced in various languages. A wide range of psychological research has demonstrated that easy processing (high processing fluency) is intrinsically experienced as positive. Due to this positive feeling, easy processing can have profound influences on preferences for names. Topolinski, Maschmann, Pecher, and Winkielman (2014) have introduced a different mechanism that influences the perception of words. Across several experiments they found that words featuring consonantal inward wanderings (inward words) were preferred over words featuring consonantal outward wanderings (outward words). They argued that this was due to the fact that approach and avoidance motivations are activated by articulating inward and outward words, because the pronunciation resembles approach and avoidance behaviors of swallowing and spitting, respectively. They suggested this close link as an underlying mechanism for the so-called in-out effect, but did not test this assumption directly. In the current work, I tested an alternative fluency account of the in-out effect. Specifically, I hypothesized that processing fluency might play a critical role instead of motivational states of approach and avoidance being necessarily activated. In Chapter 1, I introduce the general topic of my dissertation, followed by a detailed introduction of the research area of approach and avoidance motivations in Chapter 2. In Chapter 3, I narrow the topic down to orally induced approach and avoidance motivations, which is the main topic of my dissertation. In Chapter 4, I introduce the research area of ecological influences on psychological processes. This chapter builds the base for the idea that human language might serve as a source of processing fluency in the in-out effect. In the following Chapter 5, I elaborate the research area of processing fluency, for which I examined whether it plays a role in the in-out effect. After an overview of my empirical work in Chapter 6, the empirical part starts with Study 1a and Study 1b (Chapter 7) that aimed to show that two languages (Eng. & Ger.) in which the in-out effect has originally been found might feature a source of higher processing fluency for inward over outward words. The results showed that higher frequencies of inward dynamics compared to outward dynamics were found in both languages. This can lead to higher pronunciation fluency for inward compared to outward words which might in turn lay the ground for higher preferences found for inward over outward words. In Chapter 8, the assumption that inward compared to outward dynamics might be more efficient to process was tested directly in experiments that examined objective as well as subjective processing fluency of artificially constructed non-words featuring pure inward or outward dynamics. Studies 2a-4b found an objective as well as subjective processing advantage for inward over outward words. In Chapter 9, the causal role of objective and subjective pronunciation fluency in the in-out effect was examined. In Study 5 mediational analyses on item-level and across studies were conducted using objective and subjective fluency as possible mediating variables. In Study 6 mediation analyses were conducted with data on subject- and trial-level from a within-subject design. Overall, the data of the item-based, subject-based and trial-based mediation analyses provide rather mixed results. Therefore, an experimental manipulation of fluency was implemented in the last two studies. In Chapter 10, Study 7 and Study 8 demonstrate that manipulating fluency experimentally does indeed modulate the attitudinal impact of consonantal articulation direction. Articulation ease was induced by letting participants train inward or outward kinematics before the actual evaluation phase. Additionally, the simulation training was intensified in Study 8 in order to examine whether a stronger modulation of the in-out effect could be found. Training outward words led to an attenuation and, after more extensive training, even to a reversal of the in-out effect, whereas training inward words led to an enhancement of the in-out effect. This hints at my overall hypothesis that the explicit preferences of inward and outward words are, at least partially, driven by processing fluency. Almost all studies of my dissertation, except for one analysis of the item-based mediation study, speak in favor of the hypothesis that inward words compared to outward words are objectively and subjectively easier to articulate. This possibly contributes partially to a higher preference of inward over outward words. The results are discussed in Chapter 11 with respect to processing fluency and to the role of language as an ecological factor. Finally, future research ideas are elaborated. N2 - Die Namensgebung von beispielsweise Kindern oder Firmen ist meist sehr sorgfältig bedacht. Ein Name sollte sich möglichst gut anfühlen und schön klingen. So wählen weltweit agierende Firmen oft künstlich kreierte Namen, die in mehreren Sprachen leicht aussprechbar sind. Psychologische Forschung hat vielfach gezeigt, dass eine leichte Verarbeitung (hohe fluency), beispielsweise von Wörtern, implizit als positiv wahrgenommen wird. Aufgrund dieses positiven Gefühls, kann eine leichte Verarbeitung starken Einfluss auf die Präferenzen für Namen haben. Topolinski und Kollegen (2014) stellten einen anderen Mechanismus vor, der die Wahrnehmung von Wörtern beeinflussen kann. In mehreren Experimenten konnten sie zeigen, dass Wörter mit einer konsonantischen rein-Wanderung (Reinwörter) gegenüber Wörtern mit einer raus-Wanderung (Rauswörter) präferiert wurden. Sie postulieren, dass dies durch Annäherungs- und Vermeidungsmotivationen zustände käme, die durch die Artikulation von Rein- und Rauswörtern ausgelöst wurden, da das Aussprechen von diesen jeweils dem Annäherungs- und Vermeidungsverhalten im Sinne von schlucken und spucken ähneln. Die Autoren nehmen an, dass diese enge Verknüpfung von Merkmalen der Aussprache mit Annäherungs-/Vermeidungsverhalten der Mechanismus dafür ist, dass wir Rein- gegenüber Rauswörtern präferieren (Rein-Rauseffekt). Jedoch wurde diese Annahme bislang nicht direkt empirisch überprüft. In der vorliegenden Arbeit untersuche ich eine alternative fluency-Darstellung des Rein-Rauseffekts. Genauer, stelle ich die Hypothese auf, dass fluency unabhängig davon, ob Annäherungs- und Vermeidungsmotivationen aktiviert werden, eine entscheidende Rolle für den Rein-Rauseffekt spielen könnte. In Kapitel 1 führe ich das Thema meiner Dissertation ein, gefolgt von einer Vorstellung des Forschungsbereichs der Annäherungs- und Vermeidungsmotivationen (Kapitel 2). In Kapitel 3 grenze ich das Thema auf oral induzierte Motivationen ein. In Kapitel 4 stelle ich den Forschungsbereich der ökologische Einflüsse auf psychologische Prozesse vor, welches die Grundlage für meine These bildet, dass Sprache als eine fluency-Quelle im Rein-Rauseffekt fungieren könnte. In Kapitel 5 führe ich den Forschungsbereich zur fluency näher aus, da dessen Rolle im Rein-Rauseffekt in meiner Arbeit untersucht wird. Nach einem Überblick (Kapitel 6), beginnt der empirische Teil mit den Studien 1a und 1b (Kapitel 7). Diese haben untersucht, ob die zwei Sprachen (En., Deu.), in denen der Rein-Rauseffekt gefunden wurde, eine höhere fluency-Quelle für Rein- im Vergleich zu Rauswörtern darstellen können. Die Ergebnisse zeigen in beiden Sprachen ein häufigeres Vorkommen von Rein- gegenüber Rausdynamiken. Diese Ungleichverteilung der Häufigkeiten könnte eine höhere Aussprechflüssigkeit von Reinwörtern gegenüber Rauswörtern zur Folge haben, was wiederum die Grundlage für den Rein-Rauseffekt sein könnte. In Kapitel 8 wurde überprüft, ob Rein- verglichen mit Rauswörtern eine höhere fluency haben. In mehreren Experimenten wurde die objektive und subjektive fluency von künstlich konstruierten Non-Wörtern (reine Rein- oder Rausdynamiken) untersucht. Die Studien 2a-4b zeigen, dass neben der objektiven auch die subjektive fluency von Reinwörtern höher ist als die von Rauswörtern. In Kapitel 9 wurde die mögliche kausale Rolle von objektiver und subjektiver fluency auf den Rein-Rauseffekt untersucht. In Studie 5 wurden Mediationsanalysen auf Item-Ebene mit objektiver und subjektiver fluency als mögliche mediierende Variablen berechnet. In Studie 6 wurden Mediationsanalysen für subjektive fluency auf Probanden- und Trial-Ebene mit Daten aus einem Within-Subjects Design durchgeführt. Insgesamt zeigen die Analysen keine eindeutigen Befunde. Daher wurde in den letzten Studien eine experimentelle fluency-Manipulation realisiert. In Kapitel 10 zeigen Studien 7 und 8, dass eine experimentelle fluency-Manipulation Auswirkungen von konsonantischen Rein- und Rausdynamiken auf Wortpräferenzen moduliert. Die fluency wurde vor der Evaluationsphase induziert. Zusätzlich wurde das Simulationstraining in Studie 8 intensiviert, um festzustellen, ob sich eine stärkere Modulation des Rein-Rauseffektes findet. Das Trainieren von Rausdynamiken führte zu einer Abschwächung des Rein-Rauseffektes (Studie 7) und nach intensiverem Training sogar zu einer Umkehrung des Effektes (Studie 8). Das Trainieren von Reindynamiken hingegen führte zu einer Verstärkung des Rein-Rauseffektes. Diese Ergebnisse deuten darauf hin, dass Präferenzen für Rein- und Rauswörter - zumindest partiell - durch die fluency von Rein- und Rauswörtern beeinflusst sind. Nahezu alle Studien meiner Arbeit, außer der item-basierten Mediation, sprechen für meine Hypothese, dass Reinwörter gegenüber Rauswörtern sowohl subjektiv als auch objektiv leichter artikulierbar sind und möglicherweise aus diesem Grund auch präferiert werden. Die Ergebnisse werden in Kapitel 11 diskutiert. KW - Sozialpsychologie KW - Wahrnehmung KW - Präferenz KW - Wort KW - Stilles Lesen KW - Processing Fluency KW - Verarbeitungsflüssigkeit KW - Approach-Avoidance KW - Annäherung-Vermeidung KW - Wort-Präferenzen KW - Word-preferences Y1 - 2015 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-118666 ER - TY - THES A1 - Rittger, Lena T1 - Driving Behaviour and Driver Assistance at Traffic Light Intersections T1 - Fahrverhalten und Fahrerassistenz an Ampelkreuzungen N2 - The increasing importance of environmental friendly and efficient transportation guides the interest of researchers and car manufacturers towards the development of technologies that support an efficient driving style. This thesis presents the development of a traffic light assistance system with the focus on human factors. The system aims on supporting drivers in approaching traffic light intersections efficiently. In three driving simulator studies, the content related research covered the investigation of the unassisted driving task, the influence of the system on the driver’s perception of the interaction with other road users and the information strategy of the human machine interface. When the traffic light phase changes or when visibility is limited, drivers prepare driving behaviour that is not appropriate for the traffic light phase at arrival at the intersection. These situations offer the greatest potential for the assistance system. The traffic light assistant is able to change driving behaviour. However, the expectation of other road user’s emotional reactions influences driver compliance. In situations in which drivers expected to bother others with their driving behaviour, compliance to the traffic light assistant was low. Further, the deviations of driver behaviour from the target strategy of the traffic light assistant are lowest when the HMI includes the two information units target speed and action recommendations. Traffic light phase information in the HMI is a subjectively important information for drivers. The results point towards the presentation of all three information units. The method related research covered the development of a method for measuring drivers’ information demand for dynamic stimuli. While driving, specific stimuli are action relevant for drivers, i.e. they need to be processed in order to decide on the appropriate driving behaviour. Eye tracking has been the standard method for measuring information demand while driving. The novel MARS (Masking Action Relevant Stimuli) method measures information demand by masking the dynamic action relevant stimulus in the driving environment or in the vehicle. To unmask the stimulus for a fixed interval, drivers press a button at the steering wheel. In the present thesis, two driving simulator studies evaluated the MARS method. They included measuring information demand for the traffic light phasing and the in-vehicle display of the traffic light assistant. The analyses demonstrate that variations in the experimental conditions influence the information demand measured with the MARS method qualitatively similar to the influences on fixations measured by eye tracking. Due to its simple application, the MARS method represents a promising tool for transportation research. N2 - Die wachsende Bedeutung umweltfreundlicher und effizienter Mobilität hat zur zunehmenden Entwicklung von Technologien geführt, die Fahrer bei der Umsetzung eines effizienten Fahrstils unterstützen. Die vorliegende Arbeit beinhaltet die Entwicklung eines Ampelassistenten aus verkehrspsychologischer Sicht. Das System unterstützt Fahrer bei der effizienten Annäherung an Ampelkreuzungen. Drei Fahrsimulatorstudien betrachten die inhaltlichen Forschungsfragen zur Analyse von nicht-assistiertem Fahrverhalten, der Wahrnehmung der Interaktion zwischen verschiedenen Verkehrsteilnehmern mit und ohne Assistenzsystem und der Informationsstrategie in der Mensch-Maschine Schnittstelle des Systems. In Fahrsituationen mit wechselnden Ampelphasen oder Sichtverdeckung initiieren Fahrer Verhalten, das im Hinblick auf die Ampelphase bei Ankunft an der Kreuzung unangemessen ist. Diese Situationen bieten das größte Potential für eine Unterstützung durch das Assistenzsystem. Die weiteren Studien zeigen, dass der Ampelassistent das Fahrverhalten beeinflusst. Hierbei spielt die Erwartung, die Fahrer an die emotionalen Reaktionen nachfolgender Fahrer in der Kolonne haben, eine Rolle. In Situationen, in denen Fahrer erwarten andere zu behindern, sinkt die Bereitschaft sich an die Empfehlungen des Systems zu halten. Die Abweichungen des Fahrverhaltens vom Zielverhalten der Funktion sind am geringsten, wenn Handlungs- und Geschwindigkeitsempfehlungen gegeben werden. Information zur Ampelphase stellt für die Fahrer subjektiv eine wichtige Informationseinheit dar. Die Ergebnisse legen nahe, alle drei Informationen zur Kommunikation des Zielverhaltens zu präsentieren. Der methodische Teil der Arbeit beschäftigt sich mit der Messung des Informationsbedarfs für dynamische Reize. Um Entscheidungen für das angemessene Fahrverhalten zu treffen, müssen Fahrer bestimmte handlungsrelevante Informationen erfassen. Eye Tracking ist eine Standardmethode um den Informationsbedarf für fahrrelevante Reize zu messen. Die im Zuge der Arbeit entwickelte MARS (Masking Action Relevant Stimuli) Methode misst den Informationsbedarf durch Verdeckung. Der Fahrer kann die Verdeckung des Reizes durch Tastendruck für einen limitierten Zeitraum lösen. In zwei Fahrsimulatorstudien wurde die MARS Methode auf die Ampelschaltung und die Darstellung im Display des Ampelassistenten angewendet. Die Ergebnisse zeigen, dass die MARS Methode die experimentellen Variationen des Informationsbedarfs abbilden kann. Die Ergebnisse sind vergleichbar mit der Variation in Fixationen gemessen durch Eye Tracking. Aufgrund ihrer einfachen Umsetzung ist die MARS Methode als Forschungsinstrument vielversprechend. KW - Fahrerassistenzsystem KW - Verkehrsteilnehmer KW - Driver assistance KW - Traffic psychology KW - Driving simulation KW - Traffic Lights KW - Human Factors KW - Advanced Driver Assistance Systems Y1 - 2015 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-117646 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Petrasek, Tomas A1 - Prokopova, Iva A1 - Sladek, Martin A1 - Weissova, Kamila A1 - Vojtechova, Iveta A1 - Bahnik, Stepan A1 - Zemanova, Anna A1 - Schönig, Kai A1 - Berger, Stefan A1 - Tews, Bjoern A1 - Bartsch, Dusan A1 - Schwab, Martin E. A1 - Sumova, Alena A1 - Stuchlik, Ales T1 - Nogo-A-deficient transgenic rats show deficits in higher cognitive functions, decreased anxiety, and altered circadian activity patterns JF - Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience N2 - Decreased levels of Nogo-A-dependent signaling have been shown to affect behavior and cognitive functions. In Nogo-A knockout and knockdown laboratory rodents, behavioral alterations were observed, possibly corresponding with human neuropsychiatric diseases of neurodevelopmental origin, particularly schizophrenia. This study offers further insight into behavioral manifestations of Nogo-A knockdown in laboratory rats, focusing on spatial and non-spatial cognition, anxiety levels, circadian rhythmicity, and activity patterns. Demonstrated is an impairment of cognitive functions and behavioral flexibility in a spatial active avoidance task, while non-spatial memory in a step-through avoidance task was spared. No signs of anhedonia, typical for schizophrenic patients, were observed in the animals. Some measures indicated lower anxiety levels in the Nogo-A-deficient group. Circadian rhythmicity in locomotor activity was preserved in the Nogo-A knockout rats and their circadian period (tau) did not differ from controls. However, daily activity patterns were slightly altered in the knockdown animals. We conclude that a reduction of Nogo-A levels induces changes in CNS development, manifested as subtle alterations in cognitive functions, emotionality, and activity patterns. KW - AAPA KW - circadian rhythmicity KW - passive avoidance KW - Nogo-A KW - anhedonia KW - neophobia KW - morris water maze KW - place avoidance task KW - neurite outgrowth inhibitor KW - axon regeneration KW - synaptic plasticity KW - down regulation KW - traumatic brain injury KW - carousel maze KW - messenger RNA KW - genetic deletion Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-117073 VL - 8 IS - 90 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Ewald, Heike A1 - Glotzbach-Schoon, Evelyn A1 - Gerdes, Antje B. M. A1 - Andreatta, Marta A1 - Müller, Mathias A1 - Mühlberger, Andreas A1 - Pauli, Paul T1 - Delay and trace fear conditioning in a complex virtual learning environment - neural substrates of extinction JF - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience N2 - Extinction is an important mechanism to inhibit initially acquired fear responses. There is growing evidence that the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) inhibits the amygdala and therefore plays an important role in the extinction of delay fear conditioning. To our knowledge, there is no evidence on the role of the prefrontal cortex in the extinction of trace conditioning up to now. Thus, we compared brain structures involved in the extinction of human delay and trace fear conditioning in a between-subjects-design in an fMRI study. Participants were passively guided through a virtual environment during learning and extinction of conditioned fear. Two different lights served as conditioned stimuli (CS); as unconditioned stimulus (US) a mildly painful electric stimulus was delivered. In the delay conditioning group (DCG) the US was administered with offset of one light (CS+), whereas in the trace conditioning group (TCG) the US was presented 4s after CS+ offset. Both groups showed insular and striatal activation during early extinction, but differed in their prefrontal activation. The vmPFC was mainly activated in the DCG, whereas the TCG showed activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) during extinction. These results point to different extinction processes in delay and trace conditioning. VmPFC activation during extinction of delay conditioning might reflect the inhibition of the fear response. In contrast, dlPFC activation during extinction of trace conditioning may reflect modulation of working memory processes which are involved in bridging the trace interval and hold information in short term memory. KW - prefrontal cortex KW - delay conditioning KW - trace conditioning KW - extinction KW - virtual reality KW - fMRI KW - medial prefrontal cortex KW - event-related FMRI KW - orbifrontal cortex KW - contextual fear Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-116230 SN - 1662-5161 VL - 8 IS - 323 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Blechert, Jens A1 - Meule, Adrian A1 - Busch, Niko A. A1 - Ohla, Kathrin T1 - Food-pics: an image database for experimental research on eating and appetite JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - Our current environment is characterized by the omnipresence of food cues. The sight and smell of real foods, but also graphically depictions of appetizing foods, can guide our eating behavior, for example, by eliciting food craving and influencing food choice. The relevance of visual food cues on human information processing has been demonstrated by a growing body of studies employing food images across the disciplines of psychology, medicine, and neuroscience. However, currently used food image sets vary considerably across laboratories and image characteristics (contrast, brightness, etc.) and food composition (calories, macronutrients, etc.) are often unspecified. These factors might have contributed to some of the inconsistencies of this research. To remedy this, we developed food-pics, a picture database comprising 568 food images and 315 non-food images along with detailed meta-data. A total of N = 1988 individuals with large variance in age and weight from German speaking countries and North America provided normative ratings of valence, arousal, palatability, desire to eat, recognizability and visual complexity. Furthermore, data on macronutrients (g), energy density (kcal), and physical image characteristics (color composition, contrast, brightness, size, complexity) are provided. The food-pics image database is freely available under the creative commons license with the hope that the set will facilitate standardization and comparability across studies and advance experimental research on the determinants of eating behavior. Read F KW - food-cues KW - standardized food images KW - ERP KW - image properties KW - anorexia nervosa KW - restrained eaters KW - high calorie KW - brain KW - weight loss KW - visual-attention KW - responses KW - cues KW - reward KW - hunger KW - fMRI KW - eating behavior KW - obesity KW - food pictures Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-115987 SN - 1664-1078 VL - 5 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Huestegge, Lynn A1 - Rohrßen, Julia A1 - van Ermingen-Marbach, Muna A1 - Pape-Neumann, Julia A1 - Heim, Stefan T1 - Devil in the details ? Developmental dyslexia and visual long-term memory for details JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - Cognitive theories on causes of developmental dyslexia can be divided into language-specific and general accounts. While the former assume that words are special in that associated processing problems are rooted in language-related cognition (e.g., phonology) deficits, the latter propose that dyslexia is rather rooted in a general impairment of cognitive (e.g., visual and/or auditory) processing streams. In the present study, we examined to what extent dyslexia (typically characterized by poor orthographic representations) may be associated with a general deficit in visual long-term memory (LTM) for details. We compared object- and detail-related visual LTM performance (and phonological skills) between dyslexic primary school children and IQ-, age-, and gender-matched controls. The results revealed that while the overall amount of LTM errors was comparable between groups, dyslexic children exhibited a greater portion of detail-related errors. The results suggest that not only phonological, but also general visual resolution deficits in LTM may play an important role in developmental dyslexia. KW - anguage and word processing KW - picture processing KW - memory errors KW - orthographic representations KW - visual resolution deficit KW - phonology and reading KW - word form area KW - reading disability KW - spatial attention KW - ventral stream Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-115887 SN - 1664-1078 VL - 5 IS - 686 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Erlbeck, Helena A1 - Kübler, Andrea A1 - Kotchoubey, Boris A1 - Veser, Sandra T1 - Task instructions modulate the attentional mode affecting the auditory MMN and the semantic N400 JF - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience N2 - Event-related potentials (ERPs) have been proven to be a useful tool to complement clinical assessment and to detect residual cognitive functions in patients with disorders of consciousness. These ERPs are of ten recorded using passive or unspecific instructions. Patient data obtained this way are then compared to data from healthy participants, which are usually recorded using active instructions. The present study investigates the effect of attentive modulations and particularly the effect of activevs. passive instruction on the ERP components mismatch negativity (MMN) and N400. A sample of 18 healthy participants listened to three auditory paradigms: anoddball, aword priming, and a sentence paradigm. Each paradigm was presented three times with different instructions: ignoring auditory stimuli, passive listening, and focused attention on the auditory stimuli. After each task, the participants indicated their subjective effort. The N400 decreased from the focused task to the passive task, and was extinct in the ignore task. The MMN exhibited higher amplitudes in the focused and passive task compared to the ignore task. The data indicate an effect of attention on the supratemporal component of the MMN. Subjective effort was equally high in the passive and focused tasks but reduced in the ignore task. We conclude that passive listening during EEG recording is stressful and attenuates ERPs, which renders the interpretation of the results obtained in such conditions difficult. KW - ERP KW - priming KW - selective attention KW - event-related potentials KW - vigilance decrement KW - brain potentials KW - vegetative state KW - consciousness KW - component KW - predicts recovery KW - mismatch negativity KW - attention KW - instruction KW - N400 KW - MMN Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-115553 VL - 8 IS - 654 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Martens, Suzanne A1 - Bensch, Michael A1 - Halder, Sebastian A1 - Hill, Jeremy A1 - Nijboer, Femke A1 - Ramos-Murguialday, Ander A1 - Schoelkopf, Bernhard A1 - Birbaumer, Niels A1 - Gharabaghi, Alireza T1 - Epidural electrocorticography for monitoring of arousal in locked-in state JF - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience N2 - Electroencephalography (EEG) often fails to assess both the level (i.e., arousal) and the content (i.e., awareness) of pathologically altered consciousness in patients without motor responsiveness. This might be related to a decline of awareness, to episodes of low arousal and disturbed sleep patterns, and/or to distorting and attenuating effects of the skull and intermediate tissue on the recorded brain signals. Novel approaches are required to overcome these limitations. We introduced epidural electrocorticography (ECoG) for monitoring of cortical physiology in a late-stage amytrophic lateral sclerosis patient in completely locked-in state (CLIS) Despite long-term application for a period of six months, no implant related complications occurred. Recordings from the left frontal cortex were sufficient to identify three arousal states. Spectral analysis of the intrinsic oscillatory activity enabled us to extract state-dependent dominant frequencies at <4, similar to 7 and similar to 20 Hz, representing sleep-like periods, and phases of low and elevated arousal, respectively. In the absence of other biomarkers, ECoG proved to be a reliable tool for monitoring circadian rhythmicity, i.e., avoiding interference with the patient when he was sleeping and exploiting time windows of responsiveness. Moreover, the effects of interventions addressing the patient's arousal, e.g., amantadine medication, could be evaluated objectively on the basis of physiological markers, even in the absence of behavioral parameters. Epidural ECoG constitutes a feasible trade-off between surgical risk and quality of recorded brain signals to gain information on the patient's present level of arousal. This approach enables us to optimize the timing of interactions and medical interventions, all of which should take place when the patient is in a phase of high arousal. Furthermore, avoiding low responsiveness periods will facilitate measures to implement alternative communication pathways involving brain-computer interfaces (BCI). KW - temporal-lobe epilepsy KW - neuroprosthetic devices KW - brain computer interface KW - event-related potentials KW - intraoperative electrocoicography KW - electrocorticography KW - epidural recording KW - locked-in state KW - coma KW - consciousness KW - paralyzed patients KW - EEG KW - sleep KW - communication KW - frequencies KW - amyotrophic-lateral-sclerosis Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-114863 VL - 8 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Gerdes, Antje B. M. A1 - Wieser, Matthias J. A1 - Alpers, Georg W. T1 - Emotional pictures and sounds: a review of multimodal interactions of emotion cues in multiple domains JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - In everyday life, multiple sensory channels jointly trigger emotional experiences and one channel may alter processing in another channel. For example, seeing an emotional facial expression and hearing the voice's emotional tone will jointly create the emotional experience. This example, where auditory and visual input is related to social communication, has gained considerable attention by researchers. However, interactions of visual and auditory emotional information are not limited to social communication but can extend to much broader contexts including human, animal, and environmental cues. In this article, we review current research on audiovisual emotion processing beyond face-voice stimuli to develop a broader perspective on multimodal interactions in emotion processing. We argue that current concepts of multimodality should be extended in considering an ecologically valid variety of stimuli in audiovisual emotion processing. Therefore, we provide an overview of studies in which emotional sounds and interactions with complex pictures of scenes were investigated. In addition to behavioral studies, we focus on neuroimaging, electro- and peripher-physiological findings. Furthermore, we integrate these findings and identify similarities or differences. We conclude with suggestions for future research. KW - emotional sounds KW - auditory stimuli KW - facial expressions KW - human brain KW - multisensory integration KW - multimodal emotion processing KW - emotional pictures KW - audiovisual interactions KW - emotional scene stimuli KW - visual stimuli KW - social anxiety KW - human amygdala KW - attention capture KW - relevance detection KW - audiovisual integration Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-114548 SN - 1664-1078 VL - 5 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Real, Ruben G. L. A1 - Kotchoubey, Boris A1 - Kübler, Andrea T1 - Studentized continuous wavelet transform (t-CWT) in the analysis of individual ERPs: real and simulated EEG data N2 - This study aimed at evaluating the performance of the Studentized Continuous Wavelet Transform (t-CWT) as a method for the extraction and assessment of event-related brain potentials (ERP) in data from a single subject. Sensitivity, specificity, positive (PPV) and negative predictive values (NPV) of the t-CWT were assessed and compared to a variety of competing procedures using simulated EEG data at six low signal-to-noise ratios. Results show that the t-CWT combines high sensitivity and specificity with favorable PPV and NPV. Applying the t-CWT to authentic EEG data obtained from 14 healthy participants confirmed its high sensitivity. The t-CWT may thus be well suited for the assessment of weak ERPs in single-subject settings. KW - electroencephalogram KW - wavelet KW - EEG KW - t-CWT KW - significance KW - detection KW - ERP Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-113581 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Kübler, Andrea A1 - Blankertz, Benjamin A1 - Kleih, Sonja C. A1 - Kaufmann, Tobias A1 - Hammer, Eva M. T1 - Visuo-motor coordination ability predicts performance with brain-computer interfaces controlled by modulation of sensorimotor rhythms (SMR) N2 - Modulation of sensorimotor rhythms (SMR) was suggested as a control signal for brain-computer interfaces (BCI). Yet, there is a population of users estimated between 10 to 50% not able to achieve reliable control and only about 20% of users achieve high (80–100%) performance. Predicting performance prior to BCI use would facilitate selection of the most feasible system for an individual, thus constitute a practical benefit for the user, and increase our knowledge about the correlates of BCI control. In a recent study, we predicted SMR-BCI performance from psychological variables that were assessed prior to the BCI sessions and BCI control was supported with machine-learning techniques. We described two significant psychological predictors, namely the visuo-motor coordination ability and the ability to concentrate on the task. The purpose of the current study was to replicate these results thereby validating these predictors within a neurofeedback based SMR-BCI that involved no machine learning.Thirty-three healthy BCI novices participated in a calibration session and three further neurofeedback training sessions. Two variables were related with mean SMR-BCI performance: (1) a measure for the accuracy of fine motor skills, i.e., a trade for a person’s visuo-motor control ability; and (2) subject’s “attentional impulsivity”. In a linear regression they accounted for almost 20% in variance of SMR-BCI performance, but predictor (1) failed significance. Nevertheless, on the basis of our prior regression model for sensorimotor control ability we could predict current SMR-BCI performance with an average prediction error of M = 12.07%. In more than 50% of the participants, the prediction error was smaller than 10%. Hence, psychological variables played a moderate role in predicting SMR-BCI performance in a neurofeedback approach that involved no machine learning. Future studies are needed to further consolidate (or reject) the present predictors. KW - brain-computer interfaces KW - sensorimotor rhythms KW - predictors KW - visuo-motor coordination abilities KW - attentional impulsivity Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-113084 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Wieser, Matthias J. A1 - Flaisch, Tobias A1 - Pauli, Paul T1 - Raised Middle-Finger: Electrocortical Correlates of Social Conditioning with Nonverbal Affective Gestures N2 - Humans form impressions of others by associating persons (faces) with negative or positive social outcomes. This learning process has been referred to as social conditioning. In everyday life, affective nonverbal gestures may constitute important social signals cueing threat or safety, which therefore may support aforementioned learning processes. In conventional aversive conditioning, studies using electroencephalography to investigate visuocortical processing of visual stimuli paired with danger cues such as aversive noise have demonstrated facilitated processing and enhanced sensory gain in visual cortex. The present study aimed at extending this line of research to the field of social conditioning by pairing neutral face stimuli with affective nonverbal gestures. To this end, electro-cortical processing of faces serving as different conditioned stimuli was investigated in a differential social conditioning paradigm. Behavioral ratings and visually evoked steady-state potentials (ssVEP) were recorded in twenty healthy human participants, who underwent a differential conditioning procedure in which three neutral faces were paired with pictures of negative (raised middle finger), neutral (pointing), or positive (thumbs-up) gestures. As expected, faces associated with the aversive hand gesture (raised middle finger) elicited larger ssVEP amplitudes during conditioning. Moreover, theses faces were rated as to be more arousing and unpleasant. These results suggest that cortical engagement in response to faces aversively conditioned with nonverbal gestures is facilitated in order to establish persistent vigilance for social threat-related cues. This form of social conditioning allows to establish a predictive relationship between social stimuli and motivationally relevant outcomes. KW - analysis of variance KW - face KW - behavioral conditioning KW - conditioned response KW - semiotics KW - non-verbal communication KW - amygdala KW - human learning Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-113061 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Kübler, Andrea A1 - Hautzinger, Martin A1 - Ludolph, Albert A1 - Dickhaus, Thorsten A1 - Real, Ruben G. L. T1 - Well-being in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: a pilot experience sampling study N2 - Objective: The aim of this longitudinal study was to identify predictors of instantaneous well-being in patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS). Based on flow theory well-being was expected to be highest when perceived demands and perceived control were in balance, and that thinking about the past would be a risk factor for rumination which would in turn reduce well-being. Methods: Using the experience sampling method, data on current activities, associated aspects of perceived demands, control, and well-being were collected from 10 patients with ALS three times a day for two weeks. Results: Results show that perceived control was uniformly and positively associated with well-being, but that demands were only positively associated with well-being when they were perceived as controllable. Mediation analysis confirmed thinking about the past, but not thinking about the future, to be a risk factor for rumination and reduced well-being. Discussion: Findings extend our knowledge of factors contributing to well-being in ALS as not only perceived control but also perceived demands can contribute to well-being. They further show that a focus on present experiences might contribute to increased well-being. KW - amyotrophic lateral sclerosis KW - ALS KW - coping KW - well-being KW - experience sampling KW - ESM KW - reminiscence KW - rumination Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-113057 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Pfister, Roland A1 - Obhi, Sukhvinder S. A1 - Rieger, Martina A1 - Wenke, Dorit T1 - Action and perception in social contexts: intentional binding for social action effects N2 - The subjective experience of controlling events in the environment alters the perception of these events. For instance, the interval between one's own actions and their consequences is subjectively compressed—a phenomenon known as intentional binding. In two experiments, we studied intentional binding in a social setting in which actions of one agent prompted a second agent to perform another action. Participants worked in pairs and were assigned to a “leader” and a “follower” role, respectively. The leader's key presses triggered (after a variable interval) a tone and this tone served as go signal for the follower to perform a keypress as well. Leaders and followers estimated the interval between the leader's keypress and the following tone, or the interval between the tone and the follower's keypress. The leader showed reliable intentional binding for both intervals relative to the follower's estimates. These results indicate that human agents experience a pre-reflective sense of agency for genuinely social consequences of their actions. KW - intentional binding KW - action effects KW - social actions KW - action and perception KW - sense of agency Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-112828 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Meule, Adrian A1 - Hermann, Tina A1 - Kübler, Andrea T1 - A short version of the Food Cravings Questionnaire—Trait: the FCQ-T-reduced N2 - One of the most often used instruments for the assessment of food cravings is the Food Cravings Questionnaire (FCQ), which consists of a trait (FCQ-T; 39 items) and state (FCQ-S; 15 items) version. Scores on the FCQ-T have been found to be positively associated with eating pathology, body mass index (BMI), low dieting success and increases in state food craving during cognitive tasks involving appealing food stimuli. The current studies evaluated reliability and validity of a reduced version of the FCQ-T consisting of 15 items only (FCQ-T-r). Study 1 was a questionnaire study conducted online among students (N = 323). In study 2, female students (N = 70) performed a working memory task involving food and neutral pictures. Study 1 indicated a one-factorial structure and high internal consistency (α = 0.94) of the FCQ-T-r. Scores of the FCQ-T-r were positively correlated with BMI and negatively correlated with dieting success. In study 2, participants reported higher state food craving after the task compared to before. This increase was positively correlated with the FCQ-T-r. Hours since the last meal positively predicted food craving before the task when controlling for FCQ-T-r scores and the interaction of both variables. Contrarily, FCQ-T-r scores positively predicted food craving after the task when controlling for food deprivation and the interaction term. Thus, trait food craving was specifically associated with state food craving triggered by palatable food-cues, but not with state food craving related to plain hunger. Results indicate high reliability of the FCQ-T-r. Replicating studies that used the long version, small-to-medium correlations with BMI and dieting success could be found. Finally, scores on the FCQ-T-r predicted cue-elicited food craving, providing further support of its validity. The FCQ-T-r constitutes a succinct, valid and reliable self-report measure to efficiently assess experiences of food craving as a trait. KW - food carving KW - Food Carvings Questionnaire KW - psychometric properties KW - validity KW - reliability KW - body mass index KW - dieting success KW - food-cues Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-112748 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Pohl, Carsten A1 - Kunde, Wilfried A1 - Ganz, Thomas A1 - Conzelmann, Annette A1 - Pauli, Paul A1 - Kiesel, Andrea T1 - Gaming to see: action video gaming is associated with enhanced processing of masked stimuli N2 - Recent research revealed that action video game players outperform non-players in a wide range of attentional, perceptual and cognitive tasks. Here we tested if expertise in action video games is related to differences regarding the potential of shortly presented stimuli to bias behavior. In a response priming paradigm, participants classified four animal pictures functioning as targets as being smaller or larger than a reference frame. Before each target, one of the same four animal pictures was presented as a masked prime to influence participants' responses in a congruent or incongruent way. Masked primes induced congruence effects, that is, faster responses for congruent compared to incongruent conditions, indicating processing of hardly visible primes. Results also suggested that action video game players showed a larger congruence effect than non-players for 20 ms primes, whereas there was no group difference for 60 ms primes. In addition, there was a tendency for action video game players to detect masked primes for some prime durations better than non-players. Thus, action video game expertise may be accompanied by faster and more efficient processing of shortly presented visual stimuli. KW - video gaming masked stimuli KW - masked priming KW - action videogaming KW - unconscious processing KW - prime visibility KW - expertise Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-112681 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Kirsch, Wladimir A1 - Königstein, Elisabeth A1 - Kunde, Wilfried T1 - Action feedback affects the perception of action-related objects beyond actual action success N2 - Successful object-oriented action typically increases the perceived size of aimed target objects. This phenomenon has been assumed to reflect an impact of an actor's current action ability on visual perception. The actual action ability and the explicit knowledge of action outcome, however, were confounded in previous studies. The present experiments aimed at disentangling these two factors. Participants repeatedly tried to hit a circular target varying in size with a stylus movement under restricted feedback conditions. After each movement they were explicitly informed about the success in hitting the target and were then asked to judge target size. The explicit feedback regarding movement success was manipulated orthogonally to actual movement success. The results of three experiments indicated the participants' bias to judge relatively small targets as larger and relatively large targets as smaller after explicit feedback of failure than after explicit feedback of success. This pattern was independent of the actual motor performance, suggesting that the actors' evaluations of motor actions may bias perception of target objects in itself. KW - Action feedback KW - visual perception KW - action KW - knowledge of results KW - action access KW - action ability KW - perception-action coupling Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-112670 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Guhn, Anne A1 - Dresler, Thomas A1 - Andreatta, Marta A1 - Müller, Laura D. A1 - Hahn, Tim A1 - Tupak, Sara V. A1 - Polak, Thomas A1 - Deckert, Jürgen A1 - Herrmann, Martin J. T1 - Medial prefrontal cortex stimulation modulates the processing of conditioned fear N2 - The extinction of conditioned fear depends on an efficient interplay between the amygdala and the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC). In rats, high-frequency electrical mPFC stimulation has been shown to improve extinction by means of a reduction of amygdala activity. However, so far it is unclear whether stimulation of homologues regions in humans might have similar beneficial effects. Healthy volunteers received one session of either active or sham repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) covering the mPFC while undergoing a 2-day fear conditioning and extinction paradigm. Repetitive TMS was applied offline after fear acquisition in which one of two faces (CS+ but not CS−) was associated with an aversive scream (UCS). Immediate extinction learning (day 1) and extinction recall (day 2) were conducted without UCS delivery. Conditioned responses (CR) were assessed in a multimodal approach using fear-potentiated startle (FPS), skin conductance responses (SCR), functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), and self-report scales. Consistent with the hypothesis of a modulated processing of conditioned fear after high-frequency rTMS, the active group showed a reduced CS+/CS− discrimination during extinction learning as evident in FPS as well as in SCR and arousal ratings. FPS responses to CS+ further showed a linear decrement throughout both extinction sessions. This study describes the first experimental approach of influencing conditioned fear by using rTMS and can thus be a basis for future studies investigating a complementation of mPFC stimulation to cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT). KW - fear conditioning KW - memory consolidation and extinction KW - learning KW - transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) KW - medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-111309 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Pieczykolan, Aleksandra A1 - Huestegge, Lynn T1 - Oculomotor dominance in multitasking: Mechanisms of conflict resolution in cross-modal action N2 - In daily life, eye movement control usually occurs in the context of concurrent action demands in other effector domains. However, little research has focused on understanding how such cross-modal action demands are coordinated, especially when conflicting information needs to be processed conjunctly in different action modalities. In two experiments, we address this issue by studying vocal responses in the context of spatially conflicting eye movements (Experiment 1) and in the context of spatially conflicting manual actions (Experiment 2, under controlled eye fixation conditions). Crucially, a comparison across experiments allows us to assess resource scheduling priorities among the three effector systems by comparing the same (vocal) response demands in the context of eye movements in contrast to manual responses. The results indicate that in situations involving response conflict, eye movements are prioritized over concurrent action demands in another effector system. This oculomotor dominance effect corroborates previous observations in the context of multiple action demands without spatial response conflict. Furthermore, and in line with recent theoretical accounts of parallel multiple action control, resource scheduling patterns appear to be flexibly adjustable based on the temporal proximity of the two actions that need to be performed. KW - saccades KW - oculomotor dominance KW - dual-task control KW - divided attention KW - resource scheduling KW - crosstalk Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-111024 ER - TY - THES A1 - Ewald, Heike T1 - Influence of context and contingency awareness on fear conditioning – an investigation in virtual reality T1 - Der Einfluss von Kontext und Kontingenzbewusstsein auf Furchtkonditionierung – eine Untersuchung in virtueller Realität N2 - Fear conditioning is an efficient model of associative learning, which has greatly improved our knowledge of processes underlying the development and maintenance of pathological fear and anxiety. In a differential fear conditioning paradigm, one initially neutral stimulus (NS) is paired with an aversive event (unconditioned stimulus, US), whereas another stimulus does not have any consequences. After a few pairings the NS is associated with the US and consequently becomes a conditioned stimulus (CS+), which elicits a conditioned response (CR). The formation of explicit knowledge of the CS/US association during conditioning is referred to as contingency awareness. Findings about its role in fear conditioning are ambiguous. The development of a CR without contingency awareness has been shown in delay fear conditioning studies. One speaks of delay conditioning, when the US coterminates with or follows directly on the CS+. In trace conditioning, a temporal gap or “trace interval” lies between CS+ and US. According to existing evidence, trace conditioning is not possible on an implicit level and requires more cognitive resources than delay conditioning. The associations formed during fear conditioning are not exclusively associations between specific cues and aversive events. Contextual cues form the background milieu of the learning process and play an important role in both acquisition and the extinction of conditioned fear and anxiety. A common limitation in human fear conditioning studies is the lack of ecological validity, especially regarding contextual information. The use of Virtual Reality (VR) is a promising approach for creating a more complex environment which is close to a real life situation. I conducted three studies to examine cue and contextual fear conditioning with regard to the role of contingency awareness. For this purpose a VR paradigm was created, which allowed for exact manipulation of cues and contexts as well as timing of events. In all three experiments, participants were guided through one or more virtual rooms serving as contexts, in which two different lights served as CS and an electric stimulus as US. Fear potentiated startle (FPS) responses were measured as an indicator of implicit fear conditioning. To test whether participants had developed explicit awareness of the CS-US contingencies, subjective ratings were collected. The first study was designed as a pilot study to test the VR paradigm as well as the conditioning protocol. Additionally, I was interested in the effect of contingency awareness. Results provided evidence, that eye blink conditioning is possible in the virtual environment and that it does not depend on contingency awareness. Evaluative conditioning, as measured by subjective ratings, was only present in the group of participants who explicitly learned the association between CS and US. To examine acquisition and extinction of both fear associated cues and contexts, a novel cue-context generalization paradigm was applied in the second study. Besides the interplay of cues and contexts I was again interested in the effect of contingency awareness. Two different virtual offices served as fear and safety context, respectively. During acquisition, the CS+ was always followed by the US in the fear context. In the safety context, none of the lights had any consequences. During extinction, a additional (novel) context was introduced, no US was delivered in any of the contexts. Participants showed enhanced startle responses to the CS+ compared to the CS- in the fear context. Thus, discriminative learning took place regarding both cues and contexts during acquisition. This was confirmed by subjective ratings, although only for participants with explicit contingency awareness. Generalization of fear to the novel context after conditioning did not depend on awareness and was observable only on trend level. In a third experiment I looked at neuronal correlates involved in extinction of fear memory by means of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Of particular interest were differences between extinction of delay and trace fear conditioning. I applied the paradigm tested in the pilot study and additionally manipulated timing of the stimuli: In the delay conditioning group (DCG) the US was administered with offset of one light (CS+), in the trace conditioning group (TCG) the US was presented 4s after CS+ offset. Most importantly, prefrontal activation differed between the two groups. In line with existing evidence, the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) was activated in the DCG. In the TCG I found activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), which might be associated with modulation of working memory processes necessary for bridging the trace interval and holding information in short term memory. Taken together, virtual reality proved to be an elegant tool for examining human fear conditioning in complex environments, and especially for manipulating contextual information. Results indicate that explicit knowledge of contingencies is necessary for attitude formation in fear conditioning, but not for a CR on an implicit level as measured by FPS responses. They provide evidence for a two level account of fear conditioning. Discriminative learning was successful regarding both cues and contexts. Imaging results speak for different extinction processes in delay and trace conditioning, hinting that higher working memory contribution is required for trace than for delay conditioning. N2 - Furchtkonditionierung ist ein effizientes Modell für assoziatives Lernen und hat unser Wissen über Prozesse, die der Entstehung und Aufrechterhaltung von pathologischer Furcht und Angst zugrunde liegen, entscheidend vergrößert. In einem differentiellen Furchtkonditionierungparadigma wird ein zunächst neutraler Reiz (NS) gemeinsam mit einem aversiven Ereignis (unbedingter Reiz, US) dargeboten, während ein zweiter Stimulus nicht mit dem Ereignis gepaart wird. Nach mehrmaliger gemeinsamer Darbietung wird der NS mit dem US assoziiert. Dadurch wird er zu einem bedingten Reiz (CS+) und löst eine konditionierte Furchtreaktion (CR) aus. Die Bildung expliziten Wissens über die CS/US-Assoziation während der Konditionierung bezeichnet man als Kontingenzbewusstsein. Befunde über die Rolle dieses Bewusstseins in der Furchtkonditionieung sind uneinheitlich. In Delay-Furchtkonditionierungsstudien konnte die Entwicklung einer CR unabhängig von Kontingenzbewusstsein gezeigt werden. Man spricht von Delay-Konditionierung, wenn der US direkt auf den CS+ folgt. Bei der Trace-Konditionierung liegt zwischen dem CS und dem US ein kurzer zeitlicher Abstand (Trace-Interval). Für Trace-Konditionierung werden mehr kognitive Ressourcen benötigt als für Delay-Konditionierung. Auf einer impliziten Ebene ist Trace-Konditionierung nicht möglich. Die Assoziationen, die während der Furchtkonditionierung gebildet werden, beschränken sich nicht auf Assoziationen zwischen spezifischen Reizen und aversiven Ereignissen. Kontextuelle Reize bilden den Hintergrund des Lernprozesses und spielen sowohl bei der Akquisition als auch bei der Extinktion von Furcht und Angst eine wichtige Rolle. Eine häufige Einschränkung in Furchtkonditionierungsstudien beim Menschen ist der Mangel an ökologischer Validität, besonders hinsichtlich der Kontextinformationen. Der Einsatz von virtuellen Realtitäten (VR) stellt einen vielversprechenden Ansatz dar um komplexe Umgebungen nachzubilden, die nahe an Alltagssituationen sind. Um Hinweisreiz- und Kontextkonditionierung unter Berücksichtigung des Kontingenzbewusstseins zu untersuchen habe ich drei Experimente durchgeführt. Dafür wurde ein Paradigma in virtueller Realität entwickelt, das es ermöglicht, Reize, Kontexte sowie zusätzlich das Timing der Ereignisse exakt zu manipulieren. In allen drei Studien wurden Versuchspersonen durch einen oder mehrere virtuelle Räume geführt, in denen zwei verschiedene Lichter als bedingte Reize und ein elektrischer Reiz als unbedingter Reiz dienten. Furchtpotenzierte Startlereaktionen wurden gemessen als Indikator für implizite Furchtkonditionierung. Um zu überprüfen, ob die Versuchspersonen auch explizites Kontingenzbewusstsein erwoben hatten, wurden subjektive Ratings erfasst. Die erste Studie wurde als Pilotstudie konstruiert, um sowohl das VR Paradigma als auch das Konditionierungsprotokoll zu testen. Zusätzlich hat mich der Effekt des Kontingenzbewusstseins interessiert. Die Ergebnisse zeigten, dass Lidschlag-konditionierung im VR Paradigma möglich ist und dass sie nicht vom Kontingenz-bewusstsein abhängt. Allerdings war evaluative Konditionierung, gemessen durch subjektive Ratings, nur erkennbar bei Personen, die die Assoziation von CS und US explizit gelernt hatten. Um Akquisition und Extinktion sowohl furchtassoziierter Reize als auch furchtassoziierter Kontexte zu untersuchen, wurde in der zweiten Studie ein neues Reiz-Kontext-Generalisierungsparadigma eingesetzt. Neben dem Zusammenspiel von Reizen und Kontexten war ich auch hier an der Rolle des Kontingenzbewusstseins interessiert. Zwei verschiedene virtuelle Büros dienten als Furcht- bzw. Sicherheitskontext. Während der Akquisition folgte auf den CS+ im Furchtkontext immer ein US. Im Sicherheitskontext hatte keines der Lichter Konsequenzen. In der Extinktionsphase wurde zusätzlich ein neuer Kontext eingeführt. In keinem der Kontexte wurde ein US appliziert. Die Versuchspersonen reagierten nur im Furchtkontext mit erhöhter Startlereaktion auf den CS+ im Vergleich zum CS-. Diskriminatives Lernen hat sowohl hinsichtlich der Reize als auch hinsichtlich der Kontexte stattgefunden. Dies wurde bestätigt durch die subjektiven Ratings, allerdings nur bei Probanden mit Kontingenzbewusstsein. Eine Generalisierung der Angst vom Furchtkontext auf den neuen Kontext war nicht abhängig vom Kontingenzbewusstsein, konnte allerdings in der Gesamtgruppe nur tendenziell beobachtet werden. In der dritten Studie betrachtete ich neuronale Korrelate der Extinktion von Furchtgedächtnis mit Hilfe von funktioneller Magnetresonanztomographie (fMRI). Von besonderem Interesse waren dabei die Unterschiede zwischen der Extinktion von Delay- und Trace-Konditionierung. Ich habe das Paradigma aus der Pilotstudie angewendet und zusätzlich das Timing der Reize manipuliert. In der Delay-Konditionierungsgruppe (DCG) wurde der US zeitgleich mit dem Ende des CS+ appliziert, in der Trace-Konditionierungsgruppe (TCG) vier Sekunden nach Ende des CS+. Interessanterweise unterschieden sich die beiden Gruppen in ihrer präfrontalen Aktivierung. In Übereinstimmung mit der Literatur war der ventromediale Präfrontalkortex (vmPFC) in der DCG aktiviert. In der TCG konnte man Aktivierung des dorsolateralen Präfrontalkortex (dlPFC) beobachten. Dies könnte mit erhöhter Beteiligung des Arbeitsgedächtnisses zusammenhängen, die notwendig ist, um das Trace-Interval zu überbrücken und die Informationen im Kurzzeitgedächtnis zu halten. Zusammengefasst hat sich virtuelle Realität als ein elegantes Instrument zur Fuchtkonditionierung beim Menschen herausgestellt, besonders zur Manipulation von Kontextinformation. Die Ergebnisse deuten darauf hin, dass explizites Kontingenzwissen notwendig ist für evaluative Furchtkonditionierung, nicht jedoch für eine implizite CR gemessen an FPS Reaktionen. Außerdem liefern sie Evidenz für den “two level account of fear conditioning”. Die Ergebnisse der Bildgebung sprechen für zwei unterschiedliche Extinktionsprozesse bei Delay- und Trace-Konditionierung und weisen darauf hin, dass für Trace-Konditionierung eine höhere Beteiligung des Arbeitsgedächtnisses notwendig ist als für Delay-Konditionierung. KW - Klassische Konditionierung KW - Angst KW - Virtuelle Realität KW - Schreckreaktion KW - Funktionelle Kernspintomographie KW - Fear conditioning KW - virtual reality KW - contingency awareness KW - contextual conditioning KW - fMRI KW - fear potentiated startle response KW - Assoziation KW - Lernen KW - Kontingenz Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-111226 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Kübler, Andrea A1 - Holz, Elisa M. A1 - Riccio, Angela A1 - Zickler, Claudia A1 - Kaufmann, Tobias A1 - Kleih, Sonja C. A1 - Staiger-Sälzer, Pit A1 - Desideri, Lorenzo A1 - Hoogerwerf, Evert-Jan A1 - Mattia, Donatella T1 - The User-Centered Design as Novel Perspective for Evaluating the Usability of BCI-Controlled Applications N2 - Albeit research on brain-computer interfaces (BCI) for controlling applications has expanded tremendously, we still face a translational gap when bringing BCI to end-users. To bridge this gap, we adapted the user-centered design (UCD) to BCI research and development which implies a shift from focusing on single aspects, such as accuracy and information transfer rate (ITR), to a more holistic user experience. The UCD implements an iterative process between end-users and developers based on a valid evaluation procedure. Within the UCD framework usability of a device can be defined with regard to its effectiveness, efficiency, and satisfaction. We operationalized these aspects to evaluate BCI-controlled applications. Effectiveness was regarded equivalent to accuracy of selections and efficiency to the amount of information transferred per time unit and the effort invested (workload). Satisfaction was assessed with questionnaires and visual-analogue scales. These metrics have been successfully applied to several BCI-controlled applications for communication and entertainment, which were evaluated by end-users with severe motor impairment. Results of four studies, involving a total of N = 19 end-users revealed: effectiveness was moderate to high; efficiency in terms of ITR was low to high and workload low to medium; depending on the match between user and technology, and type of application satisfaction was moderate to high. The here suggested evaluation metrics within the framework of the UCD proved to be an applicable and informative approach to evaluate BCI controlled applications, and end-users with severe impairment and in the locked-in state were able to participate in this process. KW - computer software KW - event related potentials KW - games KW - head KW - head injury KW - man-computer interface KW - prototypes KW - speech Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-111051 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Meule, Adrian A1 - Beck Teran, Carina A1 - Berker, Jasmin A1 - Gründel, Tilman A1 - Mayerhofer, Martina A1 - Platte, Petra T1 - "On the differentiation between trait and state food craving: Half-year retest-reliability of the Food Cravings Questionnaire-Trait-reduced (FCQ-T-r) and the Food Cravings Questionnaire-State (FCQ-S)" N2 - Background: Food craving refers to an intense desire to consume a specific food. The Food Cravings Questionnaires (FCQs) assess food cravings on a trait and a state level. Method: The current study examined half-year retest-reliability of the Food Cravings Questionnaire-Trait-reduced (FCQ-T-r) and the Food Cravings Questionnaire-State (FCQ-S) and reports associations with current food deprivation in female students. Results: The FCQ-T-r had higher retest-reliability (rtt = .74) than the FCQ-S (rtt = .39). Although trait food craving was correlated with state food craving, it was unaffected by current food deprivation. Conclusions: Although state and trait food craving are interdependent, the FCQs are able to differentiate between the two. As scores of the FCQ-T-r represent a stable trait, but are also sensitive to changes in eating behavior, they may be useful for the investigation of the course of eating disorders and obesity. KW - Food craving KW - Food Cravings Questionnaires KW - Retest-reliability KW - University students Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-110585 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Kaufmann, Tobias A1 - Herweg, Andreas A1 - Kübler, Andrea T1 - Toward brain-computer interface based wheelchair control utilizing tactually-evoked event-related potentials N2 - Background People with severe disabilities, e.g. due to neurodegenerative disease, depend on technology that allows for accurate wheelchair control. For those who cannot operate a wheelchair with a joystick, brain-computer interfaces (BCI) may offer a valuable option. Technology depending on visual or auditory input may not be feasible as these modalities are dedicated to processing of environmental stimuli (e.g. recognition of obstacles, ambient noise). Herein we thus validated the feasibility of a BCI based on tactually-evoked event-related potentials (ERP) for wheelchair control. Furthermore, we investigated use of a dynamic stopping method to improve speed of the tactile BCI system. Methods Positions of four tactile stimulators represented navigation directions (left thigh: move left; right thigh: move right; abdomen: move forward; lower neck: move backward) and N = 15 participants delivered navigation commands by focusing their attention on the desired tactile stimulus in an oddball-paradigm. Results Participants navigated a virtual wheelchair through a building and eleven participants successfully completed the task of reaching 4 checkpoints in the building. The virtual wheelchair was equipped with simulated shared-control sensors (collision avoidance), yet these sensors were rarely needed. Conclusion We conclude that most participants achieved tactile ERP-BCI control sufficient to reliably operate a wheelchair and dynamic stopping was of high value for tactile ERP classification. Finally, this paper discusses feasibility of tactile ERPs for BCI based wheelchair control. KW - Brain-computer interface KW - Event-related potentials KW - P300 KW - Tactile KW - Wheelchair KW - Dynamic stopping Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-110042 ER - TY - THES A1 - Schmidt, Barbara T1 - Taking your chances: risk behavior and its relation to arousal, framing and motivation T1 - Die eigenen Chancen ergreifen: Risikoverhalten und dessen Beziehung zu Arousal, Framing und Motivation N2 - Human risk behavior is the subject of growing research in the field of psychology as well as economics. One central topic is the influence of psychological variables on risk behavior. Studies contained in this work investigated the impact of arousal, framing and motivation on risk behavior. Arousal can on the one hand be a temporarily stable trait and on the other hand a situation-dependent variable. We showed that low trait arousal, measured via resting heart rate, predicted risky behavior. After physical exercise, state arousal was heightened in the experiment. Participants tended to act less risky after physical exercise. Taken together, the results suggest an inverse relation of arousal and risk behavior. Most studies investigating risk behavior employ a payment method that we call pay-one method: although the gambles that are used consist of many trials, only one trial is paid out. We investigated the effect of the payment method on risk behavior by employing both the pay-one and a pay-all method, which pays out all trials, in a within-subjects design. We found that participants acted about 10% less risky in the pay-one condition compared to the pay-all condition. This result suggests that risk-aversion is over-estimated in common risk paradigms that use the pay-one method. When we worked on a hard task before, we like to engage in a more likable task afterwards. That observation led to the general classification of tasks in want-to and have-to tasks. Our body system strives towards a balance between those two task types in the sense of a homeostasis. We assessed event-related potentials (ERPs) in a risk game that we treated as a want-to task. When participants worked on a difficult have-to task before, amplitudes of the ERP-components in the risk game were raised compared to a condition where participants worked on an easy task before. We conclude that the motivation shift towards a want-to task after a have-to task can be assessed via ERP amplitudes. In conclusion, it was shown that arousal, framing and motivation are important psychological variables that influence risk behavior. The specific mechanisms of these influences have been investigated and discussed. N2 - Menschliches Risikoverhalten ist zunehmend Gegenstand psychologischer und ökonomischer Forschung. Eine zentrale Rolle spielt dabei der Einfluss psychologischer Variablen auf Risikoverhalten. Die in dieser Arbeit vorgestellten Studien untersuchen den Einfluss von physiologischer Erregung, Framing und Motivation auf Risikoverhalten. Physiologische Erregung kann einerseits als zeitlich stabile Eigenschaft, andererseits als situationsabhängige Variable gesehen werden. Wir konnten zeigen, dass niedrige zeitlich stabile physiologische Erregung, die über die Ruhe-Herzrate gemessen wurde, riskantes Verhalten vorhersagte. Nach körperlichem Training war die physiologische Erregung der Versuchspersonen im Experiment kurzzeitig erhöht. Die Versuchspersonen tendierten dazu, nach diesem Training weniger riskant zu handeln. Zusammengenommen legen die Ergebnisse nahe, dass eine inverse Relation zwischen physiologischer Erregung und Risikoverhalten vorliegt. Die meisten Studien, die sich mit Risikoverhalten befassen, verwenden die sogenannte pay-one Auszahlungsmethode: In Risikospielen, die aus vielen Durchgängen bestehen, wird nur ein Durchgang des gesamten Risikospiels ausbezahlt. Wir untersuchten den Effekt der Auszahlungsmethode auf das Risikoverhalten, indem wir sowohl die pay-one Methode als auch die pay-all Methode, bei der die Gewinne aller Durchgänge ausbezahlt werden, in einem Zwischensubjekt-Design verwendeten. Wir fanden heraus, dass die Versuchspersonen in der pay-one Bedingung etwa 10% weniger riskant handelten, verglichen mit der pay-all Bedingung. Dieses Ergebnis legt nahe, dass die Risiko-Aversion in Paradigmen, die die pay-one Methode verwenden, überschätzt wird. Nachdem wir an einer schwierigen Aufgabe gearbeitet haben, sehnen wir uns nach einer angenehmeren Aufgabe. Diese Beobachtung führte zu einer generellen Unterscheidung von want-to und have-to Aufgaben. Unser Körper strebt nach einem Gleichgewicht zwischen diesen beiden Aufgabentypen im Sinne einer Homöostase. Wir erfassten ereigniskorrelierte Potentiale (ERPs) durch das Elektroencephalogramm in einem Risikospiel, das wir als want-to Aufgabe verwendeten. Wenn Versuchspersonen vorher eine schwere Aufgabe bearbeitet hatten, waren die Amplituden der ERP- Komponenten im Risikospiel erhöht im Vergleich zu einer Bedingung, in der die Versuchspersonen vorher eine einfache Aufgabe bearbeitet hatten. Wir schlussfolgern, dass die Motivationsänderung nach einer have-to Aufgabe in Richtung einer want-to Aufgabe mit Hilfe von ERP Amplituden erfasst werden kann. Zusammenfassend wurde gezeigt, dass physiologische Erregung, Framing und Motivation wichtige psychologische Variablen sind, die Risikoverhalten beeinflussen. Die spezifischen Wirkungsweisen dieser Einflüsse wurden untersucht und diskutiert. KW - Risikoverhalten KW - Motivation KW - arousal KW - framing KW - risk behavior Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-107149 ER - TY - THES A1 - Meule, Adrian T1 - Food craving as a central construct in the self-regulation of eating behavior T1 - Craving nach Nahrungsmitteln als zentrales Konstrukt bei der Selbst-Regulation des Essverhaltens N2 - Background: Food craving refers to an intense desire to consume a specific kind of food of which chocolate is the most often craved one. It is this intensity and specificity that differentiates food craving from feelings of hunger. Although food craving and hunger often co-occur, an energy deficit is not a prerequisite for experiencing food craving, that is, it can also occur without being hungry. Food craving often precedes and predicts over- or binge eating which makes it a reasonable target in the treatment of eating disorders or obesity. One of the arguably most extensively validated measures for the assessment of food craving are the Food Cravings Questionnaires (FCQs), which measure food craving on a state (FCQ-S) and trait (FCQ-T) level. Specifically, the FCQ-S measures the intensity of current food craving whereas the FCQ-T measures the frequency of food craving experiences in general. The aims of the present thesis were to provide a German measure for the assessment of food craving and to investigate cognitive, behavioral, and physiological correlates of food craving. For this purpose, a German version of the FCQs was presented and its reliability and validity was evaluated. Using self-reports, relationships between trait food craving and dieting were examined. Cognitive-behavioral correlates of food craving were investigated using food-related tasks assessing executive functions. Psychophysiological correlates of food craving were investigated using event-related potentials (ERPs) in the electroencephalogram and heart rate variability (HRV). Possible intervention approaches to reduce food craving were derived from results of those studies. Methods: The FCQs were translated into German and their psychometric properties and correlates were investigated in a questionnaire-based study (articles #1 & #2). The relationship between state and trait food craving with executive functioning was examined with behavioral tasks measuring working memory performance and behavioral inhibition which involved highly palatable food-cues (articles #3 & #4). Electrophysiological correlates of food craving were tested with ERPs during a craving regulation task (article #5). Finally, a pilot study on the effects of HRV-biofeedback for reducing food craving was conducted (article #6). Results: The FCQs demonstrated high internal consistency while their factorial structure could only partially be replicated. The FCQ-T also had high retest-reliability which, expectedly, was lower for the FCQ-S. Validity of the FCQ-S was shown by positive relationships with current food deprivation and negative affect. Validity of the FCQ-T was shown by positive correlations with related constructs. Importantly, scores on the subscales of the FCQ-T were able to discriminate between non-dieters and successful and unsuccessful dieters (article #1). Furthermore, scores on the FCQ-T mediated the relationship between rigid dietary control strategies and low dieting success (article #2). With regard to executive functioning, high-calorie food-cues impaired working memory performance, yet this was independent of trait food craving and rarely related to state food craving (article #3). Behavioral disinhibition in response to high-calorie food-cues was predicted by trait food craving, particularly when participants were also impulsive (article #4). Downregulation of food craving by cognitive strategies in response to high-calorie food-cues increased early, but not later, segments of the Late Positive Potential (LPP) (article #5). Few sessions of HRV-biofeedback reduced self-reported food cravings and eating and weight concerns in high trait food cravers (article #6). Conclusions: The German FCQs represent sound measures with good psychometric properties for the assessment of state and trait food craving. Although state food craving increases during cognitive tasks involving highly palatable food-cues, impairment of task performance does not appear to be mediated by current food craving experiences. Instead, trait food craving is associated with low behavioral inhibition in response to high-calorie food-cues, but not with impaired working memory performance. Future studies need to examine if trait food craving and, subsequently, food-cue affected behavioral inhibition can be reduced by using food-related inhibition tasks as a training. Current food craving and ERPs in response to food-cues can easily be modulated by cognitive strategies, yet the LPP probably does not represent a direct index of food craving. Finally, HRV-biofeedback may be a useful add-on element in the treatment of disorders in which food cravings are elevated. To conclude, the current thesis provided measures for the assessment of food craving in German and showed differential relationships between state and trait food craving with self-reported dieting behavior, food-cue affected executive functioning, ERPs and HRV-biofeedback. These results provide promising starting points for interventions to reduce food craving based on (1) food-cue-related behavioral trainings of executive functions, (2) cognitive craving regulation strategies, and (3) physiological parameters such as HRV-biofeedback. N2 - Hintergrund: Craving nach Essen bezeichnet ein starkes Verlangen ein bestimmtes Nahrungsmittel zu konsumieren, welches sich meist auf Schokolade bezieht. Diese Intensität und Spezifität unterscheidet Craving nach Nahrungsmitteln von generellen Hungergefühlen. Obwohl Craving und Hunger häufig Hand in Hand gehen, ist ein Energiedefizit keine Voraussetzung dafür, Craving zu erleben; das bedeutet, es kann ebenfalls auftreten, obwohl man nicht hungrig ist. Craving geht häufig Überessen oder Essanfällen voraus, was es zu einem plausiblen Ansatzpunkt in der Therapie von Essstörungen und Adipositas macht. Eines der wohl am umfangreichsten validierten Maße zur Erfassung von Craving nach Nahrungsmitteln sind die Food Cravings Questionnaires (FCQs), die Craving als momentanen Zustand (engl. state, FCQ-S) und überdauerndes Merkmal (engl. trait, FCQ-T) erfassen. Genauer gesagt misst der FCQ-S die Intensität von aktuellem Craving nach Nahrungsmitteln, während der FCQ-T die Häufigkeit des Erlebens von Craving im Allgemeinen erfasst. Die Ziele der vorliegenden Arbeit waren die Bereitstellung eines deutschen Fragebogens zur Erfassung von Craving nach Nahrungsmitteln und die Untersuchung von kognitiven, behavioralen, und physiologischen Korrelaten von Craving. Hierfür wurde eine deutsche Version der FCQs präsentiert und deren Reliabilität und Validität überprüft. Zusammenhänge zwischen der Häufigkeit des Erlebens von Craving und Diätverhalten wurden mit Selbstberichtsfragebögen getestet. Kognitiv-behaviorale Korrelate von Craving wurden anhand von essensbezogenen Aufgaben zur Erfassung von exekutiven Funktionen untersucht. Psychophysiologische Korrelate von Craving wurden anhand von ereigniskorrelierten Potentialen (EKPs) im Elektroenzephalogramm und der Herzratenvariabilität (HRV) erforscht. Mögliche Interventionsansätze zur Reduktion von Craving wurden aus den Ergebnissen dieser Studien abgeleitet. Methoden: Die FCQs wurden auf Deutsch übersetzt und deren psychometrische Eigenschaften und Korrelate wurden in einer Fragebogenstudie untersucht (Artikel #1 & #2). Der Zusammenhang zwischen momentanem und habituellem Craving mit exekutiven Funktionen wurde anhand von behavioralen Aufgaben zur Erfassung der Arbeitsgedächtnisleistung und Verhaltenshemmung, die sehr schmackhafte Essensreize enthielten, überprüft (Artikel #3 & #4). Elektrophysiologische Korrelate von Craving wurden mit Hilfe von EKPs während einer Aufgabe zur Regulation von Craving getestet (Artikel #5). Schließlich wurde eine Pilotstudie zu den Effekten von HRV-Biofeedback zur Reduktion von Craving durchgeführt (Artikel #6). Ergebnisse: Die FCQs zeigten eine sehr hohe interne Konsistenz, wohingegen deren Faktorenstruktur nur teilweise repliziert werden konnte. Der FCQ-T hatte außerdem eine hohe Retest-Reliabilität, welche erwartungsgemäß geringer für den FCQ-S ausfiel. Validität des FCQ-S zeigte sich durch positive Zusammenhänge mit aktueller Essensdeprivation und momentaner negativer Stimmung. Validität des FCQ-T zeigte sich durch positive Korrelationen mit verwandten Konstrukten. Zudem konnte anhand der Subskalen des FCQ-T zwischen nicht-diäthaltenden und erfolgreichen und erfolglosen diäthaltenden Personen diskriminiert werden (Artikel #1). Weiterhin zeigte sich, dass der Zusammenhang zwischen rigiden Diätstrategien und geringem Diäterfolg durch Werte des FCQ-T vermittelt wurde (Artikel #2). Bezüglich der exekutiven Funktionen ergab sich, dass die Arbeitsgedächtnisleistung durch das darbieten von hochkalorischen Essensreizen vermindert wurde, allerdings war dies unabhängig von habituellem Craving und stand kaum mit momentanem Craving in Verbindung (Artikel #3). Häufiges Erleben von Craving konnte dahingegen eine behaviorale Enthemmung als Reaktion auf hochkalorische Essensreize vorhersagen, insbesondere wenn die Teilnehmer ebenfalls eine hohe Impulsivität berichteten (Artikel #4). Die Herunterregulierung von Craving während der Präsentation hochkalorischer Essensreize mit Hilfe kognitiver Strategien führte zu einer Erhöhung von früheren, nicht jedoch von späteren, Abschnitten des Late Positive Potential (LPP) (Artikel #5). Bereits wenige Übungssitzungen von HRV-Biofeedback resultierten in einer Reduktion selbstberichteter Cravings sowie essens- und gewichtsbezogener Sorgen bei Menschen mit häufigem Erleben von Cravings (Artikel #6). Schlussfolgerungen: Die deutschen FCQs stellen brauchbare Maße mit guten psychometrischen Eigenschaften zur Erfassung von Craving auf aktueller und habitueller Ebene dar. Obwohl momentanes Craving während kognitiven Aufgaben, die sehr schmackhafte Essensreize beinhalten, ansteigt, scheint eine Verminderung der Aufgabenleistung nicht durch das momentane Erleben solcher Cravings vermittelt zu sein. Habituelles Erleben von Craving ist dahingegen mit einer geringen Inhibitionsleistung auf hochkalorische Essensreize assoziiert, allerdings nicht mit einer eingeschränkten Arbeitsgedächtnisleistung. Zukünftige Studien sollten sich der Frage widmen, ob häufiges Erleben von Craving, und entsprechend auch der Einfluss von Essensreizen auf die Verhaltenshemmung, durch essensbezogene Inhibitionstrainings reduziert werden kann. Momentanes Craving und EKPs während der Präsentation von Essensreizen können leicht durch kognitive Strategien moduliert werden. Die LPP stellt hier wohl allerdings kein direktes Maß des Erlebens von Craving dar. Zu guter Letzt könnte HRV-Biofeedback ein nützliches, zusätzliches Therapieelement bei Störungen, die mit häufigem und intensivem Craving nach Nahrungsmitteln einhergehen, darstellen. Zusammenfassend lässt sich sagen, dass die vorliegende Arbeit Instrumente zur Erfassung von Craving nach Nahrungsmitteln im deutschen Sprachraum bereitgestellt hat und differenzielle Zusammenhänge zwischen momentanem und habituellem Craving mit selbstberichtetem Diätverhalten, durch Essensreize beeinträchtige exekutive Funktionen, EKPs und HRV-Biofeedback, aufgezeigt wurden. Diese Ergebnisse stellen vielversprechende Ansatzpunkte für Interventionen zur Reduktion von Craving nach Nahrungsmitteln dar, die auf (1) essensbezogenen Verhaltenstrainings der exekutiven Funktionen, (2) kognitiven Craving-Regulationsstrategien, und (3) physiologischen Parametern wie beispielsweise HRV-Biofeedback, basieren. KW - Craving KW - Essgewohnheit KW - Nahrung KW - food craving KW - Heißhunger KW - eating behavior KW - self-regulation KW - Essverhalten KW - Selbstregulation Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-104597 ER - TY - THES A1 - Dignath, David T1 - Conflict Management T1 - Konfliktmanagement N2 - Menschen haben die beeindruckende Fähigkeit zu planen, sich Ziele zu setzen und entsprechend zu handeln. Dies ist aber nicht immer der Fall. Jeder kennt Situationen, in denen Impulse, wie zum Beispiel der Drang noch ein weiteres Bier zu trinken oder stark überlernte Verhaltensgewohnheiten, wie zum Beispiel automatisch auf der rechten Straßenseite zu fahren unseren eigentlichen Zielen im Weg stehen. Dieser ´Wettstreit´ zwischen impulsiven oder habituellen Verhaltenstendenzen und zielgerichteten Handlungen wird auch als Konflikt bezeichnet. Solch ein Konflikt tritt ständig in Erscheinung und kann viele Formen annehmen. Daher überrascht es nicht weiter, dass die Art und Weise wie Konflikt kontrolliert wird, auch sehr unterschiedlich sein kann. Es scheint offensichtlich, dass Menschen Konflikte ganz verschieden handhaben können: Wenn wir eine Konfliktsituation erwarten, können wir bereits vorausschauend zusätzliche Anstrengungen unternehmen um den Konflikt dann in der Situation adäquat zu lösen. Alternativ können wir solche Situationen auch bereits im Vorfeld vermeiden und umgehen damit die Gefahr, unseren Impulsen nachzugeben. Auch wenn wir mit einer Konfliktsituation bereits konfrontiert sind, können wir versuchen durch besondere Anstrengungen diesen Konflikt aufzulösen. Schließlich haben wir auch die Möglichkeit, uns aus der Situation zurückziehen wodurch wir das Risiko minimieren, entgegen unserer eigentlichen Ziele zu handeln. Um diese Variabilität von Konfliktbewältigung besser zu verstehen, versucht die vorliegende Arbeit eine genauere Beschreibung dieser Kontrollprozesse zu geben. Zwei Dimensionen von Kontrollfunktionen werden identifiziert. Diese ergeben sich aus teilweise antagonistischen Erfordernissen an erfolgreiche Handlungskontrolle und beschreiben zum einen die Abwägung zwischen Flexibilität und Stabilität, zum Anderen die Abwägung zwischen antizipativer Selektion und reaktiver Korrektur. Um zu erklären, wie diese beiden Dimensionen von Kontrollfunktionen interagieren und wie daraus ein adäquater Umgang mit Konflikten entstehen kann, wird das „Conflict Management Framework“ eingeführt. Eine Hypothese, die aus diesem Rahmenmodel hervorgeht, beschreibt eine spezifische Strategie mit Konflikt umzugehen, die in dieser Art noch nicht untersucht wurde: Wenn Konflikt erlebt wird, ziehen sich Personen aus dieser Situation zurück und versuchen dadurch dem Konflikt zu entgehen. Der empirische Teil der Arbeit untersucht diese Verhaltensstrategie in Bezug auf Konflikterleben und testet, ob Personen unter bestimmten Bedingungen sich aus Konfliktsituationen zurückziehen. Im Rahmen dieser Untersuchung werden drei Reihen an Experimenten vorgestellt die sowohl Freie-Wahl Paradigmen, Klassifikationsaufgaben unter Zeitdruck, als auch die Aufzeichnung kontinuierlicher Bewegungen umfassen. Es zeigte sich, dass Konflikt motivationale Vermeidungstendenzen hervorruft (Experimente 1 und 2), Entscheidungsverhalten verzerrt, so dass kurzfristig mit Konflikt assoziierte Aufgaben vermieden werden (Experimente 3 und 5), und die Ausführung von komplexen Handlungen beeinflusst (Experimente 6 und 7). Diese Ergebnisse unterstützen das vorgeschlagene Rahmenmodell und ermöglichen eine vertiefte Auseinandersetzung mit der Frage, wie unterschiedliche Konfliktbewältigungsstrategien integriert werden können. Dazu wird ein konnektionistisches Modell vorgestellt, dass die parallele Anwendung von zwei unterschiedlichen Strategien zur Konfliktbewältigung ermöglicht und damit die empirischen Befunde der Experimente 3, 4 und 5 erklären kann. Im verbleibenden Teil der vorliegenden Arbeit werden Fehler einer solchen Integration von Konfliktbewältigungsstrategien analysiert. Es wird diskutiert, in wie fern das vorgeschlagene Rahmenmodell mit spezifischen klinischen Störungen vereinbar ist. Außerdem wird darauf eingegangen, wie interindividuelle Unterschiede hinsichtlich der Art und Weise Konflikte zu meistern, mit Hilfe eines solches Modells besser erklärt werden könnten. Schließlich wird ein Versuch unternommen, so genannte Selbstkontroll-Fehler durch eine mangelhafte Dynamik unterschiedlicher Konfliktbewältigungsstrategien zu erklären. N2 - Humans have a remarkable ability to plan ahead, set goals for the future and then to act accordingly. Unfortunately, this is not always the case. Everybody has experienced situations in which motivational urges like a tendency to drink another beer, or over-learned behavioral routines like driving on the right side of the road collide with ones´ goals. This tug of war between impulsive or habitual action tendencies and goal-directed actions is called a conflict. Conflict is ubiquitous and comes in many different ways. Not surprisingly, the means to control conflict are diverse, too. Clearly, people can manage conflict in multiple ways: When expecting a conflict situation to occur in the future, one can recruit more effort to resolve the conflict, for instance by inhibiting unwanted urges or habits. Alternatively one can avoid the conflict situation and thereby circumvent possible failures to control habits and impulses. Furthermore, when currently facing a conflict, people can mobilize more effort to overcome the conflict. Alternatively they can withdraw from the conflict situation to minimize the risk of indulging in their impulses and habits. To account for these different ways to master a conflict, the present thesis takes an initial step towards a characterization of the variability of control. To this aim, two dimensions of control will be identified that result from partially incompatible constraints on action control. These dimensions depict a trade-off between flexibility and stability and between anticipatory early selection and reactive late correction of control parameters. To describe how these control trade-offs interact and to explain how conflict is handled to ensure adaptation behavior, the conflict management framework is proposed. A corollary of this framework suggests that one strategy to control conflict comprises of a tendency to withdraw from a conflict situation. The empirical part probed this behavioral response to conflict and tested whether participants withdraw from conflict situations. To approach this hypothesis, three series of experiments are presented that employ free choice paradigms, speeded response classification tasks and continuous movement tracking tasks to reveal withdrawal from conflict. Results show that conflict caused motivational avoidance tendencies (Experiment 1 &2), biased decision making away from conflict tasks (Experiment 3 & 5) and affected the execution of more complex courses of action (Experiment 6 & 7). The results lend support for the proposed conflict management framework and provide the ground for a more thorough treatment of how the different conflict strategies can be integrated. As a first step, a connectionist model is presented that accounts for the simultaneous implementation of two conflict strategies observed in Experiments 3 – 5. The remainder of the present thesis analyses failures to integrate different conflict strategies. It is discussed how the conflict management framework can shed light on selected psychopathologies, inter-individual differences in control and break-downs of self-control. KW - Konflikt KW - Kognitive Kontrolle KW - Aufgabenwechsel KW - Cognitive control KW - task switching KW - Konfliktbewältigung KW - Reaktion KW - Verhaltenskontrolle KW - Exekutive Funktionen Y1 - 2014 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-103586 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Macht, Michael A1 - Janke, Wilhelm T1 - Effects of short-term energy deprivation on stress reactions in humans N2 - No abstract available. KW - Stressreaktion Y1 - 1993 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-87093 ER - TY - CHAP A1 - Krüger, Hans-Peter A1 - Lehmacher, Walter A1 - Wall, K. - D. T1 - The Fourfold Table up to N=80 T1 - Die Vierfeldtafel bis N=80 N2 - No abstract available. KW - Psychologie Y1 - 1981 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-87053 ER - TY - CHAP A1 - Körkel, Joachim A1 - Schneider, Wolfgang T1 - Domain-specific versus metacognitive knowledge effects on text recall and comprehension N2 - No abstract available. KW - Unterricht KW - Lerntheorie KW - Unterrichtspsychologie KW - Lernpsychologie KW - Pädagogische Psychologie Y1 - 1992 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-86647 ER - TY - BOOK A1 - Schneider, Wolfgang A1 - Pressley, Michael T1 - Memory development between 2 and 20 N2 - No abstract available. KW - Kind KW - Gedächtnisleistung KW - Entwicklung KW - Jugend KW - Gedächtnisbildung Y1 - 1989 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-69977 ER - TY - CHAP A1 - Krüger, Hans-Peter A1 - Gold, Rainer A1 - Hüppe, A. T1 - Coergisms between drugs and alcohol - a psychopharmacological review N2 - No abstract available. KW - Rauschgift KW - Alkohol Y1 - 1993 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-69850 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Herrmann, Martin J. A1 - Glotzbach, Evelyn A1 - Mühlberger, Andreas A1 - Gschwendtner, Kathrin A1 - Fallgatter, Andreas J. A1 - Pauli, Paul T1 - Prefrontal Brain Activation During Emotional Processing: A Functional Near Infrared Spectroscopy Study (fNIRS) JF - The Open Neuroimaging Journal N2 - The limbic system and especially the amygdala have been identified as key structures in emotion induction and regulation. Recently research has additionally focused on the influence of prefrontal areas on emotion processing in the limbic system and the amygdala. Results from fMRI studies indicate that the prefrontal cortex (PFC) is involved not only in emotion induction but also in emotion regulation. However, studies using fNIRS only report prefrontal brain activation during emotion induction. So far it lacks the attempt to compare emotion induction and emotion regulation with regard to prefrontal activation measured with fNIRS, to exclude the possibility that the reported prefrontal brain activation in fNIRS studies are mainly caused by automatic emotion regulation processes. Therefore this work tried to distinguish emotion induction from regulation via fNIRS of the prefrontal cortex. 20 healthy women viewed neutral pictures as a baseline condition, fearful pictures as induction condition and reappraised fearful pictures as regulation condition in randomized order. As predicted, the view-fearful condition led to higher arousal ratings than the view-neutral condition with the reappraise-fearful condition in between. For the fNIRS results the induction condition showed an activation of the bilateral PFC compared to the baseline condition (viewing neutral). The regulation condition showed an activation only of the left PFC compared to the baseline condition, although the direct comparison between induction and regulation condition revealed no significant difference in brain activation. Therefore our study underscores the results of previous fNIRS studies showing prefrontal brain activation during emotion induction and rejects the hypothesis that this prefrontal brain activation might only be a result of automatic emotion regulation processes. KW - fNIRS KW - Emotional processing KW - emotional regulation Y1 - 2011 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-97437 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Hommers, Wilfried A1 - Lee, Wha-Yong T1 - Unifying Kohlberg with Information Integration: The Moral Algebra of Recompense and of Kohlbergian Moral Informers JF - Psicológica N2 - In order to unify two major theories of moral judgment, a novel task is employed which combines elements of Kohlberg´s stage theory and of the theory of information integration. In contrast to the format of Kohlberg´s moral judgment interview, a nonverbal and quantitative response which makes low demands on verbal facility was used . Moral informers differing in value, i.e. high and low, are presented. The differences in effect of those two pieces of information should be substantial for a person at that specific moral stage, but small for a person at a different stage. Hence, these differences may diagnose the person's moral stage in the simplest possible way as the two levels of each of the thoughts were about typical content of the four Kohlbergian preconventional and conventional stages. The novel task allowed additionally to measure the influence of another moral concept which was about the non-Kohlbergian moral concept of recompense. After a training phase, pairs of those thoughts were presented to allow for the study of integration and individual differences. German and Korean children, 8, 10, and 12 years in age, judged deserved punishment. The patterns of means, correlations and factor loadings showed that elements of both theories can be unified, but produced unexpected results also. Additive integration of each of the two pairs of moral informers appeared, either with two Kohlbergian moral informers or with another Kohlbergian moral informer in combination with information about recompense. Also cultural independence as well as dependence, developmental changes between 8 and 10 years, and an outstanding moral impact of recompense in size and distinctiveness were observed. Y1 - 2010 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-85875 UR - http://www.uv.es/psicologica/articulos3FM.10/16Hommers.pdf ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Biehl, Stefanie C. A1 - Ehlis, Ann-Christine A1 - Müller, Laura D. A1 - Niklaus, Andrea A1 - Pauli, Paul A1 - Herrmann, Martin J. T1 - The impact of task relevance and degree of distraction on stimulus processing JF - BMC Neuroscience N2 - Background The impact of task relevance on event-related potential amplitudes of early visual processing was previously demonstrated. Study designs, however, differ greatly, not allowing simultaneous investigation of how both degree of distraction and task relevance influence processing variations. In our study, we combined different features of previous tasks. We used a modified 1-back task in which task relevant and task irrelevant stimuli were alternately presented. The task irrelevant stimuli could be from the same or from a different category as the task relevant stimuli, thereby producing high and low distracting task irrelevant stimuli. In addition, the paradigm comprised a passive viewing condition. Thus, our paradigm enabled us to compare the processing of task relevant stimuli, task irrelevant stimuli with differing degrees of distraction, and passively viewed stimuli. EEG data from twenty participants was collected and mean P100 and N170 amplitudes were analyzed. Furthermore, a potential connection of stimulus processing and symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) was investigated. Results Our results show a modulation of peak N170 amplitudes by task relevance. N170 amplitudes to task relevant stimuli were significantly higher than to high distracting task irrelevant or passively viewed stimuli. In addition, amplitudes to low distracting task irrelevant stimuli were significantly higher than to high distracting stimuli. N170 amplitudes to passively viewed stimuli were not significantly different from either kind of task irrelevant stimuli. Participants with more symptoms of hyperactivity and impulsivity showed decreased N170 amplitudes across all task conditions. On a behavioral level, lower N170 enhancement efficiency was significantly correlated with false alarm responses. Conclusions Our results point to a processing enhancement of task relevant stimuli. Unlike P100 amplitudes, N170 amplitudes were strongly influenced by enhancement and enhancement efficiency seemed to have direct behavioral consequences. These findings have potential implications for models of clinical disorders affecting selective attention, especially ADHD. KW - Selective attention KW - Working memory KW - Cognitive control KW - P100 KW - N170 KW - ADHD Y1 - 2013 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-97271 UR - http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2202/14/107 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Halder, Sebastian A1 - Ruf, Carolin Anne A1 - Furdea, Adrian A1 - Pasqualotto, Emanuele A1 - De Massari, Daniele A1 - van der Heiden, Linda A1 - Bogdan, Martin A1 - Rosenstiel, Wolfgang A1 - Birbaumer, Niels A1 - Kübler, Andrea A1 - Matuz, Tamara T1 - Prediction of P300 BCI Aptitude in Severe Motor Impairment JF - PLoS ONE N2 - Brain-computer interfaces (BCIs) provide a non-muscular communication channel for persons with severe motor impairments. Previous studies have shown that the aptitude with which a BCI can be controlled varies from person to person. A reliable predictor of performance could facilitate selection of a suitable BCI paradigm. Eleven severely motor impaired participants performed three sessions of a P300 BCI web browsing task. Before each session auditory oddball data were collected to predict the BCI aptitude of the participants exhibited in the current session. We found a strong relationship of early positive and negative potentials around 200 ms (elicited with the auditory oddball task) with performance. The amplitude of the P2 (r = −0.77) and of the N2 (r = −0.86) had the strongest correlations. Aptitude prediction using an auditory oddball was successful. The finding that the N2 amplitude is a stronger predictor of performance than P3 amplitude was reproduced after initially showing this effect with a healthy sample of BCI users. This will reduce strain on the end-users by minimizing the time needed to find suitable paradigms and inspire new approaches to improve performance. KW - amyothropic lateral sclerosis KW - electrode potentials KW - electroencephalography KW - event-related potentials KW - functional magnetic imaging KW - human performance KW - man-computer interface KW - topography Y1 - 2013 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-97268 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Kaufmann, Tobias A1 - Holz, Elisa M. A1 - Kübler, Andrea T1 - Comparison of tactile, auditory, and visual modality for brain-computer interface use: a case study with a patient in the locked-in state JF - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience N2 - This paper describes a case study with a patient in the classic locked-in state, who currently has no means of independent communication. Following a user-centered approach, we investigated event-related potentials (ERP) elicited in different modalities for use in brain-computer interface (BCI) systems. Such systems could provide her with an alternative communication channel. To investigate the most viable modality for achieving BCI based communication, classic oddball paradigms (1 rare and 1 frequent stimulus, ratio 1:5) in the visual, auditory and tactile modality were conducted (2 runs per modality). Classifiers were built on one run and tested offline on another run (and vice versa). In these paradigms, the tactile modality was clearly superior to other modalities, displaying high offline accuracy even when classification was performed on single trials only. Consequently, we tested the tactile paradigm online and the patient successfully selected targets without any error. Furthermore, we investigated use of the visual or tactile modality for different BCI systems with more than two selection options. In the visual modality, several BCI paradigms were tested offline. Neither matrix-based nor so-called gaze-independent paradigms constituted a means of control. These results may thus question the gaze-independence of current gaze-independent approaches to BCI. A tactile four-choice BCI resulted in high offline classification accuracies. Yet, online use raised various issues. Although performance was clearly above chance, practical daily life use appeared unlikely when compared to other communication approaches (e.g., partner scanning). Our results emphasize the need for user-centered design in BCI development including identification of the best stimulus modality for a particular user. Finally, the paper discusses feasibility of EEG-based BCI systems for patients in classic locked-in state and compares BCI to other AT solutions that we also tested during the study. KW - brain-computer interface KW - tactile auditory and visual modality KW - locked-in syndrome KW - user-centered design KW - end-user testing KW - assistive technology Y1 - 2013 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-97079 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Herbert, Cornelia A1 - Sfärlea, Anca A1 - Blumenthal, Terry T1 - Your emotion or mine: labeling feelings alters emotional face perception—an ERP study on automatic and intentional affect labeling JF - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience N2 - Empirical evidence suggests that words are powerful regulators of emotion processing. Although a number of studies have used words as contextual cues for emotion processing, the role of what is being labeled by the words (i.e., one's own emotion as compared to the emotion expressed by the sender) is poorly understood. The present study reports results from two experiments which used ERP methodology to evaluate the impact of emotional faces and self- vs. sender-related emotional pronoun-noun pairs (e.g., my fear vs. his fear) as cues for emotional face processing. The influence of self- and sender-related cues on the processing of fearful, angry and happy faces was investigated in two contexts: an automatic (experiment 1) and intentional affect labeling task (experiment 2), along with control conditions of passive face processing. ERP patterns varied as a function of the label's reference (self vs. sender) and the intentionality of the labeling task (experiment 1 vs. experiment 2). In experiment 1, self-related labels increased the motivational relevance of the emotional faces in the time-window of the EPN component. Processing of sender-related labels improved emotion recognition specifically for fearful faces in the N170 time-window. Spontaneous processing of affective labels modulated later stages of face processing as well. Amplitudes of the late positive potential (LPP) were reduced for fearful, happy, and angry faces relative to the control condition of passive viewing. During intentional regulation (experiment 2) amplitudes of the LPP were enhanced for emotional faces when subjects used the self-related emotion labels to label their own emotion during face processing, and they rated the faces as higher in arousal than the emotional faces that had been presented in the “label sender's emotion” condition or the passive viewing condition. The present results argue in favor of a differentiated view of language-as-context for emotion processing. KW - emotion regulation KW - language-as-context KW - affect labeling KW - face processing KW - event-related brain potentials KW - social context KW - social cognition KW - perspective taking Y1 - 2013 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-97065 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Kopf, Juliane A1 - Dresler, Thomas A1 - Reicherts, Philipp A1 - Herrmann, Martin J. A1 - Reif, Andreas T1 - The Effect of Emotional Content on Brain Activation and the Late Positive Potential in a Word n-back Task JF - PLoS ONE N2 - Introduction There is mounting evidence for the influence of emotional content on working memory performance. This is particularly important in light of the emotion processing that needs to take place when emotional content interferes with executive functions. In this study, we used emotional words of different valence but with similar arousal levels in an n-back task. Methods We examined the effects on activation in the prefrontal cortex by means of functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) and on the late positive potential (LPP). FNIRS and LPP data were examined in 30 healthy subjects. Results Behavioral results show an influence of valence on the error rate depending on the difficulty of the task: more errors were made when the valence was negative and the task difficult. Brain activation was dependent both on the difficulty of the task and on the valence: negative valence of a word diminished the increase in activation, whereas positive valence did not influence the increase in activation, while difficulty levels increased. The LPP also differentiated between the different valences, and in addition was influenced by the task difficulty, the more difficult the task, the less differentiation could be observed. Conclusions Summarized, this study shows the influence of valence on a verbal working memory task. When a word contained a negative valence, the emotional content seemed to take precedence in contrast to words containing a positive valence. Working memory and emotion processing sites seemed to overlap and compete for resources even when words are carriers of the emotional content. KW - analysis of variance KW - electrode recording KW - electroencephalography KW - emotions KW - eyes KW - near-infrared spectroscopy KW - reaction time KW - working memory Y1 - 2013 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-96687 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Halder, Sebastian A1 - Varkuti, Balint A1 - Bogdan, Martin A1 - Kübler, Andrea A1 - Rosenstiel, Wolfgang A1 - Sitaram, Ranganatha A1 - Birbaumer, Niels T1 - Prediction of brain-computer interface aptitude from individual brain structure JF - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience N2 - Objective: Brain-computer interface (BCI) provide a non-muscular communication channel for patients with impairments of the motor system. A significant number of BCI users is unable to obtain voluntary control of a BCI-system in proper time. This makes methods that can be used to determine the aptitude of a user necessary. Methods: We hypothesized that integrity and connectivity of involved white matter connections may serve as a predictor of individual BCI-performance. Therefore, we analyzed structural data from anatomical scans and DTI of motor imagery BCI-users differentiated into high and low BCI-aptitude groups based on their overall performance. Results: Using a machine learning classification method we identified discriminating structural brain trait features and correlated the best features with a continuous measure of individual BCI-performance. Prediction of the aptitude group of each participant was possible with near perfect accuracy (one error). Conclusions: Tissue volumetric analysis yielded only poor classification results. In contrast, the structural integrity and myelination quality of deep white matter structures such as the Corpus Callosum, Cingulum, and Superior Fronto-Occipital Fascicle were positively correlated with individual BCI-performance. Significance: This confirms that structural brain traits contribute to individual performance in BCI use. KW - BCI KW - motor imagery KW - aptitude KW - DTI KW - fractional anisotropy Y1 - 2013 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-96558 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Pauli, Paul A1 - Glotzbach-Schoon, Evelyn A1 - Andreatta, Marta A1 - Reif, Andreas A1 - Ewald, Heike A1 - Tröger, Christian A1 - Baumann, Christian A1 - Deckert, Jürgen A1 - Mühlberger, Andreas T1 - Contextual fear conditioning in virtual reality is affected by 5HTTLPR and NPSR1 polymorphisms: effects on fear-potentiated startle JF - Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience N2 - The serotonin (5-HT) and neuropeptide S (NPS) systems are discussed as important genetic modulators of fear and sustained anxiety contributing to the etiology of anxiety disorders. Sustained anxiety is a crucial characteristic of most anxiety disorders which likely develops through contextual fear conditioning. This study investigated if and how genetic alterations of the 5-HT and the NPS systems as well as their interaction modulate contextual fear conditioning; specifically, function polymorphic variants in the genes coding for the 5-HT transporter (5HTT) and the NPS receptor (NPSR1) were studied. A large group of healthy volunteers was therefore stratified for 5HTTLPR (S+ vs. LL carriers) and NPSR1 rs324981 (T+ vs. AA carriers) polymorphisms resulting in four genotype groups (S+/T+, S+/AA, LL/T+, LL/AA) of 20 participants each. All participants underwent contextual fear conditioning and extinction using a virtual reality (VR) paradigm. During acquisition, one virtual office room (anxiety context, CXT+) was paired with an unpredictable electric stimulus (unconditioned stimulus, US), whereas another virtual office room was not paired with any US (safety context, CXT−). During extinction no US was administered. Anxiety responses were quantified by fear-potentiated startle and ratings. Most importantly, we found a gene × gene interaction on fear-potentiated startle. Only carriers of both risk alleles (S+/T+) exhibited higher startle responses in CXT+ compared to CXT−. In contrast, anxiety ratings were only influenced by the NPSR1 polymorphism with AA carriers showing higher anxiety ratings in CXT+ as compared to CXT−. Our results speak in favor of a two level account of fear conditioning with diverging effects on implicit vs. explicit fear responses. Enhanced contextual fear conditioning as reflected in potentiated startle responses may be an endophenotype for anxiety disorders. KW - 5HTTLPR KW - NPSR1 KW - gene × gene interaction KW - contextual fear conditioning KW - fear-potentiated startle Y1 - 2013 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-96516 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Platte, Petra A1 - Herbert, Cornelia A1 - Pauli, Paul A1 - Breslin, Paul A. S. T1 - Oral Perceptions of Fat and Taste Stimuli Are Modulated by Affect and Mood Induction JF - PLoS ONE N2 - This study examined the impact of three clinical psychological variables (non-pathological levels of depression and anxiety, as well as experimentally manipulated mood) on fat and taste perception in healthy subjects. After a baseline orosensory evaluation, ‘sad’, ‘happy’ and ‘neutral’ video clips were presented to induce corresponding moods in eighty participants. Following mood manipulation, subjects rated five different oral stimuli, appearing sweet, umami, sour, bitter, fatty, which were delivered at five different concentrations each. Depression levels were assessed with Beck’s Depression Inventory (BDI) and anxiety levels were assessed via the Spielberger’s STAI-trait and state questionnaire. Overall, subjects were able to track the concentrations of the stimuli correctly, yet depression level affected taste ratings. First, depression scores were positively correlated with sucrose ratings. Second, subjects with depression scores above the sample median rated sucrose and quinine as more intense after mood induction (positive, negative and neutral). Third and most important, the group with enhanced depression scores did not rate low and high fat stimuli differently after positive or negative mood induction, whereas, during baseline or during the non-emotional neutral condition they rated the fat intensity as increasing with concentration. Consistent with others’ prior observations we also found that sweet and bitter stimuli at baseline were rated as more intense by participants with higher anxiety scores and that after positive and negative mood induction, citric acid was rated as stronger tasting compared to baseline. The observation that subjects with mild subclinical depression rated low and high fat stimuli similarly when in positive or negative mood is novel and likely has potential implications for unhealthy eating patterns. This deficit may foster unconscious eating of fatty foods in sub-clinical mildly depressed populations. KW - analysis of variance KW - anxiety KW - citric acid KW - depression KW - glutamate KW - quinine KW - sensory perception KW - sucrose Y1 - 2013 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-96421 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Meule, Adrian A1 - Fath, Katharina A1 - Real, Ruben G. L. A1 - Sütterlin, Stefan A1 - Vögele, Claus A1 - Kübler, Andrea T1 - Quality of life, emotion regulation, and heart rate variability in individuals with intellectual disabilities and concomitant impaired vision JF - Psychology of Well-Being: Theory, Research and Practice N2 - Background Positive associations have been found between quality of life, emotion regulation strategies, and heart rate variability (HRV) in people without intellectual disabilities. However, emotion regulation and HRV have rarely been investigated in people with intellectual disabilities. Assessment of subjectively reported quality of life and emotion regulation strategies in this population is even more difficult when participants are also visually impaired. Methods Subjective and objective quality of life, emotion regulation strategies, and HRV at rest were measured in a sample of people with intellectual disabilities and concomitant impaired vision (N = 35). Heart rate was recorded during a 10 min resting period. For the assessment of quality of life and emotion regulation, custom made tactile versions of questionnaire-based instruments were used that enabled participants to grasp response categories. Results The combined use of reappraisal and suppression as emotion regulation strategies was associated with higher HRV and quality of life. HRV was associated with objective quality of life only. Emotion regulation strategies partially mediated the relationship between HRV and quality of life. Conclusions Results replicate findings about associations between quality of life, emotion regulation, and HRV and extend them to individuals with intellectual disabilities. Furthermore, this study demonstrated that quality of life and emotion regulation could be assessed in such populations even with concomitant impaired vision with modified tactile versions of established questionnaires. HRV may be used as a physiological index to evaluate physical and affective conditions in this population. KW - Quality of life KW - Emotion regulation KW - Heart rate variability KW - Cardiac autonomic regulation KW - Intellectual disability KW - Impaired vision Y1 - 2013 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-96247 UR - http://www.psywb.com/content/3/1/1 ER - TY - THES A1 - Kaufmann, Tobias T1 - Brain-computer interfaces based on event-related potentials: toward fast, reliable and easy-to-use communication systems for people with neurodegenerative disease T1 - Gehirn-Computer Schnittstellen basierend auf ereigniskorrelierten Potentialen: Entwicklung von schnellen, zuverlässigen und leicht zu bedienenden Kommunikationssystemen für Menschen mit neurodegenerativer Erkrankung N2 - Objective: Brain Computer Interfaces (BCI) provide a muscle independent interaction channel making them particularly valuable for individuals with severe motor impairment. Thus, different BCI systems and applications have been proposed as assistive technology (AT) solutions for such patients. The most prominent system for communication utilizes event-related potentials (ERP) obtained from the electroencephalogram (EEG) to allow for communication on a character-by-character basis. Yet in their current state of technology, daily life use cases of such systems are rare. In addition to the high EEG preparation effort, one of the main reasons is the low information throughput compared to other existing AT solutions. Furthermore, when testing BCI systems in patients, a performance drop is usually observed compared to healthy users. Patients often display a low signal-to-noise ratio of the recorded EEG and detection of brain responses may be aggravated due to internally (e.g. spasm) or externally induced artifacts (e.g. from ventilation devices). Consequently, practical BCI systems need to cope with mani-fold inter-individual differences. Whilst these high demands lead to increasing complexity of the technology, daily life use of BCI systems requires straightforward setup including an easy-to-use graphical user interface that nonprofessionals can handle without expert support. Research questions of this thesis: This dissertation project aimed at bringing forward BCI technology toward a possible integration into end-users' daily life. Four basic research questions were addressed: (1) Can we identify performance predictors so that we can provide users with individual BCI solutions without the need of multiple, demanding testing sessions? (2) Can we provide complex BCI technology in an automated, user-friendly and easy-to-use manner, so that BCIs can be used without expert support at end-users' homes? (3) How can we account for and improve the low information transfer rates as compared to other existing assistive technology solutions? (4) How can we prevent the performance drop often seen when bringing BCI technology that was tested in healthy users to those with severe motor impairment? Results and discussion: (1) Heart rate variability (HRV) as an index of inhibitory control (i.e. the ability to allocate attention resources and inhibit distracting stimuli) was significantly related to ERP-BCI performance and accounted for almost 26% of variance. HRV is easy to assess from short heartbeat recordings and may thus serve as a performance predictor for ERP-BCIs. Due to missing software solutions for appropriate processing of artifacts in heartbeat data (electrocardiogram and inter-beat interval data), our own tool was developed that is available free of charge. To date, more than 100 researchers worldwide have requested the tool. Recently, a new version was developed and released together with a website (www.artiifact.de). (2) Furthermore, a study of this thesis demonstrated that BCI technology can be incorporated into easy-to-use software, including auto-calibration and predictive text entry. Naïve, healthy nonprofessionals were able to control the software without expert support and successfully spelled words using the auto-calibrated BCI. They reported that software handling was straightforward and that they would be able to explain the system to others. However, future research is required to study transfer of the results to patient samples. (3) The commonly used ERP-BCI paradigm was significantly improved. Instead of simply highlighting visually displayed characters as is usually done, pictures of famous faces were used as stimulus material. As a result, specific brain potentials involved in face recognition and face processing were elicited. The event-related EEG thus displayed an increased signal-to-noise ratio, which facilitated the detection of ERPs extremely well. Consequently, BCI performance was significantly increased. (4) The good results of this new face-flashing paradigm achieved with healthy participants transferred well to users with neurodegenerative disease. Using a face paradigm boosted information throughput. Importantly, two users who were highly inefficient with the commonly used paradigm displayed high accuracy when exposed to the face paradigm. The increased signal-to-noise ratio of the recorded EEG thus helped them to overcome their BCI inefficiency. Significance: The presented work at hand (1) successfully identified a physiological predictor of ERP-BCI performance, (2) proved the technology ready to be operated by naïve nonprofessionals without expert support, (3) significantly improved the commonly used spelling paradigm and (4) thereby displayed a way to effectively prevent BCI inefficiency in patients with neurodegenerative disease. Additionally, missing software solutions for appropriate handling of artifacts in heartbeat data encouraged development of our own software tool that is available to the research community free of charge. In sum, this thesis significantly improved current BCI technology and enhanced our understanding of physiological correlates of BCI performance. N2 - Zielsetzung: Gehirn-Computer Schnittstellen (engl. Brain-Computer Interface, BCI) bilden einen muskel-unabhängigen Interaktionskanal, was sie besonders für Menschen mit starken, motorischen Einschränkungen wertvoll macht. Daher wurden verschiedene BCI-Systeme und -Anwendungen als unterstützende Technologien (UT) für diese Patienten vorgeschlagen. Das am häufigsten verwendete System zu Kommunikationszwecken basiert auf ereigniskorrelierten Potentialen (EKP) des Elektroenzephalogramms (EEG) und ermöglicht es Buchstabe für Buchstabe zu kommunizieren. Zum derzeitigen Stand der Technik sind Berichte über alltägliche Verwendung von BCI-Systemen jedoch selten. Zusätzlich zu dem hohen Präparationsaufwand, der mit dem Messen eines EEGs verbunden ist, ist eine der Hauptursachen der verhältnismäßig geringe Informationstransfer im Vergleich zu anderen existierenden UT-Lösungen. Zudem wird häufig beobachtet, dass das BCI-Kontrollvermögen bei Patienten deutlich niedriger ist als bei gesunden Nutzern. Das EEG von Patienten weißt häufig ein niedrigeres Signal-zu-Rausch-Verhältnis auf und die Erkennung von Hirnantworten kann durch interne (z. B. Spasmus) oder externe Artefakte (z. B. von einem Beatmungsgerät herrührend) zusätzlich erschwert werden. Somit müssen praxisrelevante BCI-Systeme mit einer Vielfalt von interindividuellen Unterschieden klar kommen. Obschon diese hohen Anforderungen zu einer zunehmend komplexeren Technologie führen, erfordert der Alltagsgebrauch von BCI-Systemen einen einfachen Aufbau inklusive leicht zu bedienender, grafischer Benutzeroberfläche, die von Laien, ohne Unterstützung von Experten, bedient werden kann. Forschungsfragestellungen dieser Dissertationsschrift: Das Dissertationsprojekt bezweckte die Weiterentwicklung von BCI-Systemen, um den Weg für eine Integration in das tägliche Leben von Benutzern zu bahnen. Vier grundlegende Forschungsfragestellungen wurden adressiert: (1) Können Prädiktoren des BCI-Kontrollvermögens gefunden werden, sodass Nutzer mit individuell angepassten BCI-Lösungen versorgt werden können, ohne dass mehrfache, anstrengende Testsitzungen notwendig sind? (2) Kann komplexe BCI-Technologie auf automatisierte, nutzerfreundliche und leicht zu bedienende Weise bereitgestellt werden, sodass BCIs, ohne die Unterstützung von Experten, zuhause verwendet werden können? (3) Wie kann man den im Vergleich zu anderen existierenden UT-Lösungen niedrigen Informationstransferraten begegnen und sie erhöhen? (4) Wie kann ein Abfall des Kontrollvermögens verhindert werden, der häufig in Erscheinung tritt, wenn man BCI-Technologie, die an Gesunden getestet wurde, zu Patienten mit starken motorischen Einschränkungen bringt? Ergebnisse und Diskussion: (1) Herzratenvariabilität als Index des inhibitorischen Kontrollvermögens (die Fähigkeit Aufmerksamkeitsressourcen bereitzustellen und ablenkende Reize zu inhibieren) wurde zu EKP-BCI-Kontrollvermögen in einen signifikanten Bezug gesetzt und klärte beinahe 26% der Varianz auf. HRV ist leicht aus kurzen Aufzeichnungen des Herzschlags zu erheben und könnte daher als Prädiktor des EKP-BCI-Kontrollvermögens dienen. Aufgrund fehlender Softwarelösungen für angemessene Artefaktbehandlung in Aufzeichnungen des Herzschlags (Elektrokardiogramm und Interbeat-Intervall Daten) wurde ein eigenes Programm entwickelt, das frei erhältlich ist. Bis heute wurde es von mehr als 100 Wissenschaftlern weltweit angefordert. Unlängst wurde zudem eine neue Version entwickelt, die zusammen mit einer Website veröffentlicht wurde (www.artiifact.de). (2) Des Weiteren zeigte eine Studie dieser Dissertation, dass BCI-Technologie samt Auto-Kalibration und Wortvervollständigung in eine leicht zu bedienende Software integriert werden kann. Ungeschulte, gesunde Probanden waren in der Lage die Software ohne Unterstützung von Experten zu bedienen und buchstabierten mit dem auto-kalibrierten BCI erfolgreich Wörter. Sie gaben an, dass die Bedienung der Software leicht zu tätigen sei und dass sie in der Lage wären, das System anderen zu erklären. Jedoch muss zukünftige Forschung klären, ob sich die Ergebnisse auf Patienten übertragen lassen. (3) Das häufig verwendete EKP-BCI-Paradigma wurde signifikant verbessert. Statt - wie normalerweise getätigt - visuell präsentierte Buchstaben einfach aufleuchten zu lassen, wurden Bilder berühmter Gesichter als Stimulationsmaterial verwendet. Hierdurch wurden spezifische Gehirnpotentiale der Gesichtserkennung und -verarbeitung ausgelöst. Das ereigniskorrelierte EEG wies daher ein höheres Signal-zu-Rausch-Verhältnis auf, was die Detektion von EKPs stark vereinfachte. Infolgedessen war das BCI-Kontrollvermögen signifikant erhöht. (4) Die guten Ergebnisse dieses neuen Gesichter-Stimulus-Paradigmas, die mit gesunden Probanden erreicht wurden, ließen sich gut auf Patienten mit neurodegenerativen Erkrankungen übertragen. Die Verwendung eines Gesichter-Paradigmas erhöhte den Informationstransfer erheblich. Zwei Nutzer, die sehr ineffizient im Umgang mit dem herkömmlichen BCI-System waren, erreichten zudem ein hohes Kontrollvermögen mit dem Gesichter-Paradigma. Das erhöhte Signal-zu-Rausch-Verhältnis des aufgezeichneten EEGs half ihnen somit ihre BCI Ineffizienz zu überwinden. Signifikanz: Die hier vorgestellte Arbeit (1) identifizierte einen physiologischen Prädiktor des EKP-BCI-Kontrollvermögens, (2) zeigte, dass die Technologie bereit ist für die Verwendung durch ungeschulte Laien ohne Unterstützung von Experten, (3) verbesserte das herkömmliche Kommunikationsparadigma signifikant, und (4) zeigte hierdurch einen Weg auf, die BCI Ineffizienz von Patienten mit neurodegenerativen Erkrankungen effektiv zu verhindern. Die fehlende Softwarelösung zur angemessenen Behandlung von Artefakten in Aufzeichnungen des Herzschlags animierte zudem zur Entwicklung einer eigenen Anwendung, die der Wissenschaftsgemeinschaft kostenfrei zur Verfügung gestellt wird. Zusammenfassend kann gesagt werden, dass dieses Dissertationsprojekt somit derzeitige BCI-Technologie signifikant verbesserte und unser Verständnis physiologischer Korrelate des BCI-Kontrollvermögens erweiterte. KW - Gehirn-Computer-Schnittstelle KW - Herzfrequenzvariabilität KW - Nervendegeneration KW - BCI KW - HRV Y1 - 2013 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-83441 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Herbert, Cornelia A1 - Kübler, Andrea A1 - Vögele, Klaus T1 - Risk for Eating Disorders Modulates Startle-Responses to Body Words N2 - Body image disturbances are core symptoms of eating disorders (EDs). Recent evidence suggests that changes in body image may occur prior to ED onset and are not restricted to in-vivo exposure (e.g. mirror image), but also evident during presentation of abstract cues such as body shape and weight-related words. In the present study startle modulation, heart rate and subjective evaluations were examined during reading of body words and neutral words in 41 student female volunteers screened for risk of EDs. The aim was to determine if responses to body words are attributable to a general negativity bias regardless of ED risk or if activated, ED relevant negative body schemas facilitate priming of defensive responses. Heart rate and word ratings differed between body words and neutral words in the whole female sample, supporting a general processing bias for body weight and shape-related concepts in young women regardless of ED risk. Startle modulation was specifically related to eating disorder symptoms, as was indicated by significant positive correlations with self-reported body dissatisfaction. These results emphasize the relevance of examining body schema representations as a function of ED risk across different levels of responding. Peripheral-physiological measures such as the startle reflex could possibly be used as predictors of females’ risk for developing EDs in the future. KW - Psychologie Y1 - 2013 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-78140 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Halder, Sebastian A1 - Hammer, Eva Maria A1 - Kleih, Sonja Claudia A1 - Bogdan, Martin A1 - Rosenstiel, Wolfgang A1 - Birbaumer, Nils A1 - Kübler, Andrea T1 - Prediction of Auditory and Visual P300 Brain-Computer Interface Aptitude N2 - Objective: Brain-computer interfaces (BCIs) provide a non-muscular communication channel for patients with late-stage motoneuron disease (e.g., amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS)) or otherwise motor impaired people and are also used for motor rehabilitation in chronic stroke. Differences in the ability to use a BCI vary from person to person and from session to session. A reliable predictor of aptitude would allow for the selection of suitable BCI paradigms. For this reason, we investigated whether P300 BCI aptitude could be predicted from a short experiment with a standard auditory oddball. Methods: Forty healthy participants performed an electroencephalography (EEG) based visual and auditory P300-BCI spelling task in a single session. In addition, prior to each session an auditory oddball was presented. Features extracted from the auditory oddball were analyzed with respect to predictive power for BCI aptitude. Results: Correlation between auditory oddball response and P300 BCI accuracy revealed a strong relationship between accuracy and N2 amplitude and the amplitude of a late ERP component between 400 and 600 ms. Interestingly, the P3 amplitude of the auditory oddball response was not correlated with accuracy. Conclusions: Event-related potentials recorded during a standard auditory oddball session moderately predict aptitude in an audiory and highly in a visual P300 BCI. The predictor will allow for faster paradigm selection. Significance: Our method will reduce strain on patients because unsuccessful training may be avoided, provided the results can be generalized to the patient population. KW - Psychologie Y1 - 2013 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-77992 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Herbort, Oliver A1 - Butz, Martin V. T1 - Too good to be true? Ideomotor theory from a computational perspective N2 - In recent years, Ideomotor Theory has regained widespread attention and sparked the development of a number of theories on goal-directed behavior and learning. However, there are two issues with previous studies’ use of Ideomotor Theory. Although Ideomotor Theory is seen as very general, it is often studied in settings that are considerably more simplistic than most natural situations. Moreover, Ideomotor Theory’s claim that effect anticipations directly trigger actions and that action-effect learning is based on the formation of direct action-effect associations is hard to address empirically. We address these points from a computational perspective. A simple computational model of Ideomotor Theory was tested in tasks with different degrees of complexity.The model evaluation showed that Ideomotor Theory is a computationally feasible approach for understanding efficient action-effect learning for goal-directed behavior if the following preconditions are met: (1) The range of potential actions and effects has to be restricted. (2) Effects have to follow actions within a short time window. (3) Actions have to be simple and may not require sequencing. The first two preconditions also limit human performance and thus support Ideomotor Theory. The last precondition can be circumvented by extending the model with more complex, indirect action generation processes. In conclusion, we suggest that IdeomotorTheory offers a comprehensive framework to understand action-effect learning. However, we also suggest that additional processes may mediate the conversion of effect anticipations into actions in many situations. KW - Psychologie KW - ideomotor theory KW - associative learning KW - computational model KW - planning KW - consolidation Y1 - 2012 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-76383 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Wieser, Mattias J. A1 - Brosch, Tobias T1 - Faces in context: A review and systematization of contextual influences on affective face processing N2 - Facial expressions are of eminent importance for social interaction as they convey information about other individuals’ emotions and social intentions. According to the predominant “basic emotion” approach, the perception of emotion in faces is based on the rapid, auto- matic categorization of prototypical, universal expressions. Consequently, the perception of facial expressions has typically been investigated using isolated, de-contextualized, static pictures of facial expressions that maximize the distinction between categories. However, in everyday life, an individual’s face is not perceived in isolation, but almost always appears within a situational context, which may arise from other people, the physical environment surrounding the face, as well as multichannel information from the sender. Furthermore, situational context may be provided by the perceiver, including already present social infor- mation gained from affective learning and implicit processing biases such as race bias.Thus, the perception of facial expressions is presumably always influenced by contextual vari- ables. In this comprehensive review, we aim at (1) systematizing the contextual variables that may influence the perception of facial expressions and (2) summarizing experimental paradigms and findings that have been used to investigate these influences. The studies reviewed here demonstrate that perception and neural processing of facial expressions are substantially modified by contextual information, including verbal, visual, and auditory information presented together with the face as well as knowledge or processing biases already present in the observer. These findings further challenge the assumption of auto- matic, hardwired categorical emotion extraction mechanisms predicted by basic emotion theories. Taking into account a recent model on face processing, we discuss where and when these different contextual influences may take place, thus outlining potential avenues in future research. KW - Psychologie KW - facial expression KW - face perception KW - emotion KW - context KW - “basic emotion” Y1 - 2012 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-76351 ER - TY - THES A1 - Schubert, Lisa T1 - The Respective Impact of Stimulus Valence and Processing Fluency on Evaluative Judgments in Stereotype Disconfirmation T1 - Der relative Einfluss von Stimulusvalenz und Verarbeitungsflüssigkeit auf evaluative Urteile im Stereotypkontext N2 - Both specific stimulus valence and unspecific processing dynamics can influence evaluative responses. Eight experiments investigated their respective influence on evaluative judgments in the domain of stereotyping. Valence of stereotypic information and consistency-driven fluency were manipulated in an impression formation paradigm. When information about the to-be-evaluated target person was strongly valenced, no effects of consistency-driven fluency were observed. Higher cognitive processes, valence of inconsistent attributes, processing priority of category information, and impression formation instructions were ruled out as possible factors responsible for the non-occurrence of fluency effects. However, consistency-driven fluency did influence the evaluative judgment, if the information about a target person was not strongly valenced. It is therefore concluded that both stimulus valence and consistency-driven processing fluency play a role in evaluative judgments in the domain of stereotyping. The respective impact of stimulus valence is much stronger than the impact of unspecific processing dynamics, however. Implications for fluency research and the applied field of stereotype change are discussed. N2 - Sowohl Stimulusvalenz als auch unspezifische Verarbeitungsflüssigkeit können evaluative Urteile beeinflussen. In acht Experimenten wurde ihr relativer Einfluss im Stereotypkontext untersucht. Hierzu wurden in einem Eindrucksbildungsparadigma die Valenz von stereotypisierender Information und die konsistenzbasierte Verarbeitungsflüssigkeit manipuliert. Im Falle starker Stimulusvalenz der Information über die zu bewertende Person hatte konsistenzbasierte Verarbeitungsflüssigkeit keinen Einfluss auf das evaluative Urteil. Höhere kognitive Prozesse, Valenz der inkonsistenten Eigenschaften, Dominanz von kategorialer Information und Eindrucksbildungsinstruktionen konnten als mögliche Erklärungen für das Ausbleiben von Effekten der Verarbeitungsflüssigkeit ausgeschlossen werden. Konsistenzbasierte Verarbeitungsflüssigkeit hatte allerdings einen Einfluss auf evaluative Urteile, wenn Stimuli keine starke Wertigkeit aufwiesen. Daraus wird geschlossen, dass sowohl Stimulusvalenz als auch unspezifische Verarbeitungsflüssigkeit bei evaluativen Urteilen im Stereotypkontext eine Rolle spielen. Der relative Einfluss von Stimulusvalenz ist jedoch deutlich stärker als der Einfluss von Verarbeitungsflüssigkeit. Implikationen für Theorien der Verarbeitungsflüssigkeit und für die Anwendung im Bereich der Stereotypveränderung werden diskutiert. KW - Vorurteil KW - Eindrucksbildung KW - Verarbeitungsflüssigkeit KW - Stereotype KW - Psychology KW - Person Perception KW - Fluency KW - Stereotypes KW - Informationsverarbeitung KW - Psychologie Y1 - 2012 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-77426 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Haering, Carola A1 - Kiesel, Andrea T1 - Mine is earlier than yours: Causal beliefs influence the perceived time of action effects N2 - When a key press causes a stimulus, the key press is perceived later and the stimulus earlier than key presses and stimuli presented independently. This bias in time perception has been linked to the intention to produce the effect and thus been called intentional binding (IB). In recent studies it has been shown that the IB effect is stronger when participants believed that they caused the effect stimulus compared to when they believed that another person caused the effect (Desantis et al., 2011). In this experiment we ask whether causal beliefs influence the perceived time of an effect when the putative effect occurs temporally close to another stimulus that is also an effect. In our study two participants performed the same task on connected computers with separate screens. Each trial started synchro- nously on both computers. When a participant pressed a key, a red and a yellow stimulus appeared as action effects simultaneously or with a slight delay of up to 50 ms. The partic- ipants’ task was to judge the temporal order of these two effect stimuli. Participants were either told that one participant caused one of the two stimuli while the other participant seated at the other computer caused the other stimulus, or each participant was told that he/she caused both stimuli. The different causal beliefs changed the perceived time of the effects’ appearance relative to each other. When participants believed they each caused one effect, their “own” effect was perceived earlier than the other participant’s effect. When the participants believed each caused both effects, no difference in the perceived temporal order of the red and yellow effect was found. These results confirm that higher order causal beliefs change the perceived time of an action effect even in a setting in which the occurrence of the putative effect can be directly compared to a reference stimulus. KW - Psychologie KW - intentional binding KW - causal belief KW - causality KW - temporal order judgments KW - TOJ KW - agency Y1 - 2012 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-76229 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Janczyk, Markus A1 - Heinemann, Alexander A1 - Pfister, Roland T1 - Instant attraction: Immediate action-effect bindings occur for both, stimulus- and goal-driven actions N2 - Flexible behavior is only possible if contingencies between own actions and following environmental effects are acquired as quickly as possible; and recent findings indeed point toward an immediate formation of action-effect bindings already after a single coupling of an action and its effect. The present study explored whether these short-term bindings occur for both, stimulus- and goal-driven actions (“forced-choice actions” vs. “free-choice actions”). Two experiments confirmed that immediate action-effect bindings are formed for both types of actions and affect upcoming behavior. These findings support the view that action-effect binding is a ubiquitous phenomenon which occurs for any type of action. KW - Psychologie KW - ideomotor theory KW - action planning KW - free-choice KW - forced-choice KW - action-effects KW - binding Y1 - 2012 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-76203 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Herbert, Cornelia A1 - Sütterlin, Stefan T1 - Do not respond! Doing the think/no-think and go/no-go tasks concurrently leads to memory impairment of unpleasant items during later recall N2 - Previous research using neuroimaging methods proposed a link between mechanisms controlling motor response inhibition and suppression of unwanted memories.The present study investigated this hypothesis behaviorally by combining the think/no-think paradigm (TNT) with a go/no-go motor inhibition task. Participants first learned unpleasant cue-target pairs. Cue words were then presented as go or no-go items in the TNT. Participants’ task was to respond to the cues and think of the target word aloud or to inhibit their response to the cue and the target word from coming to mind. Cued recall assessed immediately after the TNT revealed reduced recall performance for no-go targets compared to go targets or baseline cues not presented in the TNT. The results demonstrate that doing the no-think and no-go task concurrently leads to memory suppression of unpleasant items during later recall. Results are discussed in line with recent empirical research and theoretical positions. KW - Psychologie KW - memory suppression KW - emotion KW - response inhibition KW - go/no-go task KW - think/no-think paradigm Y1 - 2012 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-76028 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Roeser, Karolin A1 - Eichholz, Ruth A1 - Schwerdtle, Barbara A1 - Schlarb, Angelika A. A1 - Kübler, Andrea T1 - Relationship of sleep quality and health-related quality of life in adolescents according to self- and proxy ratings: a questionnaire survey N2 - Introduction: Sleep disturbances are common in adolescents and adversely affect performance, social contact, and susceptibility to stress. We investigated the hypothesis of a relationship between sleep and health-related quality of life (HRQoL), and applied self- and proxy ratings. Materials and Methods: The sample comprised 92 adolescents aged 11–17 years. All participants and their parents completed a HRQoL measure and the Sleep Disturbance Scale for Children (SDSC ). Children with SDSC T -scores above the normal range (above 60) were classified as poor sleepers. Results: According to self- and proxy ratings, good sleepers reported significantly higher HRQoL than poor sleep- ers. Sleep disturbances were significantly higher and HRQoL significantly lower in self- as compared to parental ratings. Parent-child agreement was higher for subscales measuring observable aspects. Girls experienced significantly stronger sleep disturbances and lower self-rated HRQoL than boys. Discussion: Our findings support the positive relationship of sleep and HRQoL. Furthermore, parents significantly underestimate sleep disturbances and overestimate HRQoL in their children. KW - Psychiatrie KW - quality of life KW - sleep KW - adolescence KW - parent-child agreement KW - sleep disorders Y1 - 2012 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-75953 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Fazel-Rezai, Reza A1 - Allison, Brendan Z. A1 - Guger, Christoph A1 - Sellers, Eric W. A1 - Kleih, Sonja C. A1 - Kübler, Andrea T1 - P300 brain computer interface: current challenges and emerging trends N2 - A brain-computer interface (BCI) enables communication without movement based on brain signals measured with electroencephalography (EEG). BCIs usually rely on one of three types of signals: the P300 and other components of the event-related potential (ERP), steady state visual evoked potential (SSVEP), or event related desynchronization (ERD). Although P300 BCIs were introduced over twenty years ago, the past few years have seen a strong increase in P300 BCI research. This closed-loop BCI approach relies on the P300 and other components of the ERP, based on an oddball paradigm presented to the subject. In this paper, we overview the current status of P300 BCI technology, and then discuss new directions: paradigms for eliciting P300s; signal processing methods; applications; and hybrid BCIs. We conclude that P300 BCIs are quite promising, as several emerging directions have not yet been fully explored and could lead to improvements in bit rate, reliability, usability, and flexibility. KW - Psychologie KW - brain computer interface KW - P300 KW - event-related potential Y1 - 2012 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-75941 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Likowski, Katja U. A1 - Mühlberger, Andreas A1 - Gerdes, Antje B. M. A1 - Wieser, Mattias J. A1 - Pauli, Paul A1 - Weyers, Peter T1 - Facial mimicry and the mirror neuron system: simultaneous acquisition of facial electromyography and functional magnetic resonance imaging N2 - Numerous studies have shown that humans automatically react with congruent facial reactions, i.e., facial mimicry, when seeing a vis-á-vis’ facial expressions. The current experiment is the first investigating the neuronal structures responsible for differences in the occurrence of such facial mimicry reactions by simultaneously measuring BOLD and facial EMG in an MRI scanner. Therefore, 20 female students viewed emotional facial expressions (happy, sad, and angry) of male and female avatar characters. During picture presentation, the BOLD signal as well as M. zygomaticus major and M. corrugator supercilii activity were recorded simultaneously. Results show prototypical patterns of facial mimicry after correction for MR-related artifacts: enhanced M. zygomaticus major activity in response to happy and enhanced M. corrugator supercilii activity in response to sad and angry expressions. Regression analyses show that these congruent facial reactions correlate significantly with activations in the IFG, SMA, and cerebellum. Stronger zygomaticus reactions to happy faces were further associated to increased activities in the caudate, MTG, and PCC. Corrugator reactions to angry expressions were further correlated with the hippocampus, insula, and STS. Results are discussed in relation to core and extended models of the mirror neuron system (MNS). KW - Psychologie KW - mimicry KW - EMG KW - fMRI KW - mirrorneuronsystem Y1 - 2012 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-75813 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Tobias, Kaufmann A1 - Völker, Stefan A1 - Gunesch, Laura A1 - Kübler, Andrea T1 - Spelling is just a click away – a user-centered brain-computer interface including auto-calibration and predictive text entry N2 - Brain–computer interfaces (BCI) based on event-related potentials (ERP) allow for selection of characters from a visually presented character-matrix and thus provide a communica- tion channel for users with neurodegenerative disease. Although they have been topic of research for more than 20 years and were multiply proven to be a reliable communication method, BCIs are almost exclusively used in experimental settings, handled by qualified experts. This study investigates if ERP–BCIs can be handled independently by laymen without expert support, which is inevitable for establishing BCIs in end-user’s daily life situations. Furthermore we compared the classic character-by-character text entry against a predictive text entry (PTE) that directly incorporates predictive text into the character- matrix. N = 19 BCI novices handled a user-centered ERP–BCI application on their own without expert support. The software individually adjusted classifier weights and control parameters in the background, invisible to the user (auto-calibration). All participants were able to operate the software on their own and to twice correctly spell a sentence with the auto-calibrated classifier (once with PTE, once without). Our PTE increased spelling speed and, importantly, did not reduce accuracy. In sum, this study demonstrates feasi- bility of auto-calibrating ERP–BCI use, independently by laymen and the strong benefit of integrating predictive text directly into the character-matrix. KW - Psychologie KW - P300-Speller KW - ERP-BCI KW - brain–computerinterface KW - user-centered KW - auto-calibration KW - predictivetextentry KW - event-relatedpotentials KW - assisitvetechnology Y1 - 2012 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-75739 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Schwerdtle, Barbara A1 - Kanis, Julia A1 - Kahl, Lena A1 - Kübler, Andrea A1 - Schlarb, Angelika A. T1 - Children’s Sleep Comic: development of a new diagnostic tool for children with sleep disorders [original research] N2 - Background: A solid diagnosis of sleep disorders in children should include both self-ratings and parent ratings. However, there are few standardized self-assessment instruments to meet this need. The Children’s Sleep Comic is an adapted version of the unpublished German questionnaire “Freiburger Kinderschlafcomic” and provides pictures for items and responses. Because the drawings were outdated and allowed only for qualitative analysis, we revised the comic, tested its applicability in a target sample, and suggest a procedure for quantitative analysis. Methods: All items were updated and pictures were newly drawn. We used a sample of 201 children aged 5–10 years to test the applicability of the Children’s Sleep Comic in young children and to run a preliminary analysis. Results: The Children’s Sleep Comic comprises 37 items covering relevant aspects of sleep disorders in children. Application took on average 30 minutes. The procedure was well accepted by the children, as reflected by the absence of any dropouts. First comparisons with established questionnaires indicated moderate correlations. Conclusion: The Children’s Sleep Comic is appropriate for screening sleep behavior and sleep problems in children. The interactive procedure can foster a good relationship between the investigator and the child, and thus establish the basis for successful intervention if necessary. KW - Psychologie KW - children KW - sleep KW - sleep disorders KW - diagnostic KW - assessment KW - self-rating Y1 - 2012 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-75722 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Kirsch, Wladimir A1 - Herbort, Oliver A1 - Butz, Martin V. A1 - Kunde, Wilfried T1 - Influence of Motor Planning on Distance Perception within the Peripersonal Space N2 - We examined whether movement costs as defined by movement magnitude have an impact on distance perception in near space. In Experiment 1, participants were given a numerical cue regarding the amplitude of a hand movement to be carried out. Before the movement execution, the length of a visual distance had to be judged. These visual distances were judged to be larger, the larger the amplitude of the concurrently prepared hand movement was. In Experiment 2, in which numerical cues were merely memorized without concurrent movement planning, this general increase of distance with cue size was not observed. The results of these experiments indicate that visual perception of near space is specifically affected by the costs of planned hand movements. KW - Psychologie Y1 - 2012 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-75332 ER - TY - THES A1 - Reuß, Heiko T1 - The interplay of unconscious processing and cognitive control T1 - Das Zusammenspiel unbewusster Verarbeitung und kognitiver Kontrolle N2 - The aim of this study was both to investigate the influence of cognitive control on unconscious processing, and to investigate the influence of unconscious processing on cognitive control. At first, different mechanisms and accounts to explain unconscious priming are presented. Here, perceptual and motor processes, as well as stimulus-response learning, semantic categorization, and the action trigger account as theories to explain motor priming are discussed. Then, the issue of the potential limits of unconscious processing is presented. Findings that indicate that active current intentions and expertise modulate unconscious processing are illustrated. Subsequently, results that imply an influence of unconsciously presented stimuli that goes beyond motor processes are discussed, with a special focus on inhibition processes, orienting of attention, task set activation, and conflict adaptation. Then I present the results of my own empirical work. Experiment 1 shows that the effective processing of unconsciously presented stimuli depends on expertise, even when potentially confounding difference between the expert and novice groups are controlled. The results of Experiments 2 and 3 indicate that the intention to use particular stimuli is a crucial factor for the effectiveness of these stimuli when they are presented unconsciously. Additionally, these findings show that shifts of attention can be triggered by centrally presented masked arrow cues. Experiments 4 and 5 broaden these results to cue stimuli that are not inherently associated with a spatial meaning. The finding corroborate that typically endogenously controlled shifts of attention can also be induced by unconscious stimuli. Experiments 6 and 7 demonstrate that even a central cognitive control process like task set activation is not contingent on conscious awareness, but can in contrast be triggered through unconscious stimulation. Finally, these results are integrated and I discuss how the concept of cognitive control and the limits of unconscious processing may have to be reconsidered. Furthermore, potential future research possibilities in this field are presented. N2 - Das Ziel dieser Arbeit war es, sowohl den Einfluss von kognitiver Kontrolle auf unbewusste Verarbeitung, als auch den Einfluss unbewusster Verarbeitung auf kognitive Kontrolle zu untersuchen. Zunächst werden verschiedene Mechanismen und Ansätze zur Erklärung unbewusster Bahnung vorgestellt. Dabei werden perzeptuelle Prozesse sowie motorische Prozesse beleuchtet und mit Reiz-Reaktions-Verbindungen, semantischer Kategorisierung und dem Ansatz handlungsdeterminierender Reizerwartungen drei verschiedene Ansätze zur Erklärung motorischer Bahnung besprochen. Danach wird die Problematik der Grenzen unbewusster Verarbeitung dargestellt. Es werden Befunde vorgestellt, die Hinweise auf den Einfluss von aktiven Aufgabeneinstellungen sowie von Expertise auf unbewusste Verarbeitung geben. Als nächstes werden Ergebnisse besprochen, die einen über motorische Prozesse hinausgehenden Einfluss unbewusster Reize nahelegen. Dabei wird insbesondere auf den Einfluss auf Hemmprozesse, Aufmerksamkeitsausrichtung, die Aktivierung von Aufgabeneinstellungen und Konfliktadaptation eingegangen. Dann werden die Ergebnisse eigener empirischer Arbeiten vorgestellt. In Experiment 1 wurde gezeigt, dass die effektive Verarbeitung unbewusster Reize von Expertise abhängt, auch wenn sonstige Unterschiede zwischen Experten- und Novizen-Gruppen kontrolliert sind. Die Ergebnisse von Experiment 2 und 3 zeigten, dass die Absicht, bestimmte Reize zu nutzen, ein entscheidender Faktor dabei ist, ob diese Reize auch unbewusst einen Effekt entfalten können. Zudem wurde hier gezeigt, dass Aufmerksamkeitsverschiebungen durch zentral präsentierte, maskierte Pfeile ausgelöst werden können. Die Experimente 4 und 5 erweiterten diesen Befund auf Hinweisreize, die keine inhärente räumliche Bedeutung haben. Hier konnte bestätigt werden, dass eigentlich endogen gesteuerte Aufmerksamkeitsverschiebungen durch unbewusste Reize induziert werden können. Die Experimente 6 und 7 zeigten, dass selbst ein zentraler kognitiver Kontrollprozess wie die Aktivierung verschiedener Aufgabeneinstellungen nicht bewusstseinspflichtig ist, sondern im Gegenteil durch unbewusste Stimulation in Gang gesetzt werden kann. Letztendlich werden diese Ergebnisse zueinander in Beziehung gesetzt. Es wird diskutiert, inwiefern das Konzept kognitiver Kontrolle und die Grenzen unbewusster Verarbeitung neu betrachtet werden müssen. Außerdem werden mögliche zukünftige Forschungsfelder in diesem Bereich aufgezeigt. KW - Bewusstsein KW - unconscious processing KW - cognitive control KW - Kognitiver Prozess KW - Allgemeine Psychologie Y1 - 2013 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-76950 ER - TY - THES A1 - Kinateder, Max T1 - Social Influence in Emergency Situations – Studies in Virtual Reality T1 - Sozialer Einfluss in Notfallsituationen - Studien in virtueller Realität N2 - In 1999, a tragic catastrophe occurred in the Mont Blanc Tunnel, one of the most important transalpine road tunnels. Twenty-seven of the victims never left their vehicles as a result of which they were trapped in smoke and suffocated (Beard & Carvel, 2005). Immediate evacuation is crucial in tunnel fires, but still many tunnel users stay passive. During emergency situations people strongly influence each other’s behavior (e.g. Nilsson & Johansson, 2009a). So far, only few empirical experimental studies investigated the interaction of individuals during emergencies. Recent developments of advanced immersive virtual worlds, allow simulating emergency situations which makes analogue studies possible. In the present dissertation project, theoretical aspects of human behavior and SI in emergencies are addressed (Chapter 1). The question of Social Influence in emergency situations is investigated in five simulation studies during different relevant stages of the evacuation process from a simulated road tunnel fire (Chapter 2). In the last part, the results are discussed and criticized (Chapter 3). Using a virtual reality (VR) road tunnel scenario, study 1 (pilot study) and 2 investigated the effect of information about adequate behavior in tunnel emergencies as well as Social Influence (SI) on drivers’ behavior. Based on a classic study of Darley and Latané (1968) on bystander inhibition, the effect of passive bystanders on self-evacuation was analyzed. Sixty participants were confronted with an accident and smoke in a road tunnel. The presence of bystanders and information status was manipulated and consequently, participants were randomly assigned into four different groups. Informed participants read a brochure containing relevant information about safety behavior in emergency situations prior to the tunnel drives. In the bystander conditions, passive bystanders were situated in a car in front of the emergency situation. Participants who had received relevant information left the car more frequently than the other participants. Neither significant effect of bystanders nor interaction with information status on the participants’ behavior was observed. Study 3 (pilot study) examined a possible alternative explanation for weak SI in VR. Based on the Threshold Theory of Social Influence (Blascovich, 2002b) and the work of Guadagno et al. (2007), the perception of virtual humans as an avatar (a virtual representation of a real human being) or as an agent (a computer-controlled animated character) was manipulated. Subsequently, 32 participants experienced an accident similar to the one in study 1. However, they were co-drivers and a virtual agent (VA) was the driver. Participants reacted differently in avatar and agent condition. Consequently, the manipulation of the avatar condition was implemented in study 4. In study 4, SI within the vehicle was investigated, as drivers are mostly not alone in their car. In a tunnel scenario similar to the first study, 34 participants were confronted with an emergency situation either as drivers or co-drivers. In the driver group, participants drove themselves and a VA was sitting on the passenger seat. Correspondently, participants in the co-driver group were seated on the passenger seat and the VA drove the vehicle on a pre-recorded path. Like in study 1, the tunnel was blocked by an accident and smoke was coming from the accident in one drive. The VA initially stayed inactive after stopping the vehicle but started to evacuate after ca. 30 seconds. About one third of the sample left the vehicle during the situation. There were no significant differences between drivers and co-drivers regarding the frequency of leaving the vehicle. Co-drivers waited significantly longer than drivers before leaving the vehicle. Study 5 looked at the pre-movement and movement phase of the evacuation process. Forty participants were repeatedly confronted with an emergency situation in a virtual road tunnel filled with smoke. Four different experimental conditions systematically varied the presence and behavior of a VA. In all but one conditions a VA was present. Across all conditions at least 60% of the participants went to the emergency exit. If the VA went to the emergency exit, the ratio increased to 75%. If the VA went in the opposite direction of the exit, however, only 61% went there. If participants were confronted with a passive VA, they needed significantly longer until they started moving and reached the emergency exit. The main and most important finding across all studies is that SI is relevant for self-evacuation, but the degree of SI varies across the phases of evacuation and situation. In addition to the core findings, relevant theoretical and methodological questions regarding the general usefulness and limitations of VR as a research tool are discussed. Finally, a short summary and outlook on possible future studies is presented. N2 - In der Mont Blanc Tunnel Katastrophe im Jahr 1999 starben 39 Menschen, von denen 27 nicht versucht hatten rechtzeitig zu flüchten. In der Folge wurden diese Personen vom Rauch eingeschlossen und erstickten in ihren Fahrzeugen. Bisher gibt es nur vereinzelt empirische Studien, die sich mit Fragestellungen zu menschlichem Verhalten in Gefahrensituationen beschäftigen. Noch weniger Arbeiten beschäftigen sich mit der gegenseitigen Beeinflussung von Individuen in Gefahrensituationen. Die wohl wahrscheinlichste Erklärung ist, dass es bisher kaum möglich oder zu aufwändig war, Gefahrensituationen experimentalpsychologisch zu untersuchen. Die Entwicklung immersiver virtueller Welten erlaubt es allerdings, solche Situationen ökologisch valide zu simulieren. Erstes Ziel des Promotionsvorhabens war deshalb sozialen Einfluss in virtuell simulierten Gefahrensituationen mittels experimentalpsychologischer Studien zu untersuchen. Zweites Ziel war die Untersuchung methodischer Grundlagen zur Untersuchung von sozialem Einfluss in virtueller Realität. Die Dissertation gliedert sich in drei Teile: Kapitel 1 führt zunächst in die Themen menschliches Verhalten in Gefahrensituationen, Evakuierung und sozialer Einfluss während Notfällen ein. In Kapitel 2 werden die eigenen empirischen Arbeiten dargestellt. Dabei wurde sozialer Einfluss in verschiedenen kritischen Phasen des Evakuierungsprozesses während eines Tunnelbrandes untersucht. Insgesamt wurden fünf unabhängige Erhebungen mit insgesamt 194 Studienteilnehmern durchgeführt. Studie 1 (Vorstudie) und 2 untersuchte den sozialen Einfluss passiver virtueller Bystander sowie den Effekt von Informationen auf das Fluchtverhalten. Die Probanden wurden mit einem Unfall und sich ausbreitendem Rauch in einem Straßentunnel konfrontiert. In einer Probandengruppe befanden sich passive Bystander am Unfallort. Die Ergebnisse zeigten erstens, dass nur wenige uninformierte Probanden überhaupt das Fahrzeug verließen um aus sich zum Notausgang zu begeben. Zweitens, konnten Information das Verhalten während des Unfalls verbessern. Drittens fand sich nur ein schwacher Einfluss passiver virtueller Bystander auf das Verhalten der Probanden in der Notfallsituation. Studie 3 (Vorstudie) untersuchte eine mögliche alternative Erklärung für schwachen sozialen Einfluss in virtueller Realität. Hier wurde die Wahrnehmung virtueller Menschen als Avatar (eine von realen Menschen gesteuerte virtuelle Repräsentation) oder als Agent (vom Computer gesteuerte animierte Figuren) manipuliert. Anschließend erlebten die Probanden einen ähnlichen Unfall wie in Studie 1. Allerdings waren sie nun Beifahrer und erlebten den Unfall gemeinsam mit einem animierten virtuellen Menschen der das Fahrzeug lenkte. Probanden ließen sich eher von einer animierten Menschen beeinflussen, wenn sie überzeugt waren, dass es sich um einen Avatar handelt. Studie 4 untersuchte den Einfluss von anderen Personen im Fahrzeug auf das Verhalten in einer Notfallsituation. Dabei erlebten die Probanden die gleiche Gefahrensituation wie in Studie 1 entweder als Fahrer oder als Beifahrer. Gleichzeitig befand sich ein virtueller Agent im Fahrzeug, der sich zunächst passiv verhielt aber nach einer gewissen Zeit das Fahrzeug verließ. Es zeigte sich, dass Probanden zügiger dem Verhalten des virtuellen Agenten folgten, wenn der Agent Fahrer und die Probanden Beifahrer waren. In Studie 5 wurde das eigentliche Evakuierungsverhalten während eines simulierten Tunnelbrandes untersucht. Dabei befanden sich die Probanden wiederholt in einem stark verrauchten Tunnel und das Verhalten eines virtuellen Agenten wurde systematisch manipuliert. Die meisten Probanden suchten den Notausgang auf, jedoch zeigte sich, dass das Verhalten des virtuellen Agenten die Probanden beeinflusste: Ging der Agent in die entgegengesetzte Richtung des Notausgangs oder blieb dieser passiv, so gingen die Probanden seltener zum Notausgang und benötigten signifikant länger um diesen zu erreichen. Kapitel 3 enthält schließlich die Zusammenfassung und Diskussion der Studien. Dabei werden die Ergebnisse der Arbeit in den aktuellen Stand der Forschung eingeordnet, praktische Implikationen abgeleitet und der weitere Forschungsbedarf beschrieben. Insgesamt konnte gezeigt werden, dass sozialer Einfluss in Gefahrensituationen von Bedeutung ist, aber während verschiedener Phasen des Evakuierungsprozesses unterschiedlich stark ist. Abschließen werden die theoretischen und methodischen Kritikpunkte der Forschungsarbeiten genannt und erörtert. KW - Notfall KW - Sozialpsychologie KW - Sozialer Einfluss KW - Psychologische Sicherheitsforschung KW - Strassentunnel KW - Evakuierung KW - Virtuelle Realitaet KW - Social Influence KW - Tunnel emergencies KW - safety research KW - evacuation behavior KW - virtual reality Y1 - 2012 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-76805 ER - TY - THES A1 - Shiban, Youssef T1 - Attenuating Renewal following Exposure Therapy : Mechanisms of Exposure in Multiple Contexts and its Influence on the Renewal of Fear: Studies in Virtual Reality T1 - Abmilderung von Renewal Effekten nach Expositionstherapie N2 - „Renewal“ bezeichnet das Wiederauftreten von Angst nach erfolgreicher Expositionstherapie in Folge einer erneuten Konfrontation mit dem phobischen Stimulus in einem neuen, sich vom Expositionskontext unterscheidenden Kontext. Bouton (1994) zufolge deutet diese Angstrückkehr durch einen Kontextwechsel darauf hin, dass die Angst nicht gelöscht wurde. Stattdessen wurde während der Expositionssitzung eine neue Assoziation gelernt, die das gefürchtete Objekt mit „keiner Angst“, also den konditionierten Reiz (conditioned stimulus, CS) mit „keinem unkonditionierten Reiz“ (no unconditioned stimulus, no US), verbindet. Bouton argumentiert weiter, dass diese Assoziation kontextabhängig ist, wodurch Effekte wie Angst-Renewal erklärt werden können. Da in einem neuen Kontext die CS-no US-Assoziation nicht aktiviert wird, wird die Angst auch nicht gehemmt. Die Kontextabhängigkeit der CS-no US-Assoziation wurde in mehreren Studien belegt (Balooch & Neumann, 2011; Siavash Bandarian Balooch, Neumann, & Boschen, 2012; Culver, Stoyanova, & Craske, 2011; Kim & Richardson, 2009; Neumann & Kitlertsirivatana, 2010). Aktuell konzentriert sich die Forschung zur Therapie von Angststörungen auf die Frage, wie Angst reduziert und gleichzeitig ein Rückfall verhindert werden kann. Hierzu werden verschiedene Expositionsprotokolle untersucht, wie zum Beispiel (1) Exposition in mehreren Kontexten (multiple contexts exposure, MCE), um Renewal zu reduzieren (z.B. Balooch & Neumann, 2011); (2) verlängerte Exposition (prolonged exposure, PE), um die hemmende Assoziation während des Extinktionslernes zu stärken (z.B. Thomas, Vurbic, & Novak, 2009) und (3) Rekonsolidierungs-Updates (reconsolidation update, RU), die den Rekonsolidierungsprozess durch eine kurze Exposition des CS+ vor der eigentlichen Exposition aktualisieren sollen (Schiller et al., 2010). Bisher liegen jedoch nur sehr wenige Studien vor, die diese neuen Expositionsprotokolle an klinischen Stichproben untersucht haben, und - soweit bekannt - keine Studie, welche die Wirkmechanismen dieser Protokolle an einer klinischen Stichprobe erforscht. Die vorliegende Dissertation hat drei Ziele. Das erste Ziel besteht darin zu prüfen, ob Expositionstherapie in multiplen Kontexten die Wahrscheinlichkeit von Renewal reduziert. Das zweite Ziel ist die Untersuchung der Mechanismen, die dem Effekt der Exposition in multiplen Kontexten zugrunde liegen und das dritte ist den Kontext im Zusammenhang mit Konditionierung und Extinktion zu konzeptualisieren. Insgesamt wurden drei Studien durchgeführt. Die erste Studie untersuchte den Effekt von Exposition in multiplen Kontexten auf Renewal, die zweite und dritte Studie die Wirkmechanismen von MCE. In der ersten Studie wurden spinnenphobische Probanden (N = 30) viermal mit einer virtuellen Spinne konfrontiert. Die Expositionstrials wurden entweder in einem gleichbleibenden Kontext oder in vier verschiedenen Kontexten durchgeführt. Am Ende der Sitzung absolvierten alle Teilnehmer einen virtuellen Renewaltest, bei dem die virtuelle Spinne in einem neuen Kontext gezeigt wurde, und einen in vivo Verhaltensvermeidungstest (behavioral avoidance test, BAT) mit einer echten Spinne. Die Ergebnisse zeigten, dass Probanden, welche die vier Expositionstrials in unterschiedlichen Kontexten erfuhren, weniger Angst, sowohl im virtuellen Renewaltest als auch im BAT, erlebten. In dieser Studie konnte die Wirksamkeit von MCE für die Reduktion von Renewal erfolgreich nachgewiesen werden. Studie 2 (N = 35) untersuchte die Wirkmechanismen von MCE in einem differentiellen Konditionierungsparadigma. Die Extinktion wurde in multiplen Kontexten durchgeführt. Hierbei war das Ziel, eine ähnliche Verminderung von Renewal wie in Studie 1 nachzuweisen. Der Extinktion folgten zwei Tests, mit dem Ziel mögliche hemmende Effekte des Kontexts, die während der Extinktionsphase erworben wurden, aufzudecken. Bezüglich des Effektes von MCE wurden drei Hypothesen aufgestellt: (1) Der Extinktionskontext wird mit der Exposition assoziiert, fungiert folglich während der Extinktion als Sicherheitssignal und konkurriert daher mit dem Sicherheitslernen des CS. Dies führt zu einem verminderten Extinktionseffekt auf den CS, wenn die Extinktion nur in einem Kontext durchgeführt wird. (2) Die Elemente im Extinktionskontext (z.B. Raumfarbe, Möbel) stehen im Zusammenhang mit der CS-no US-Assoziation und erinnern daher an die Extinktion, was zu einer größeren Angsthemmung führt, wenn sie während eines Tests gezeigt werden. (3) Nach der emotionalen Prozesstheorie (emotional process theory; Bouton, 1994; Foa et al., 1996) bestimmen die Therapieprozessfaktoren die Stärke des Renewals. Beispielsweise korrelieren initiale Angstaktivierung, Aktivierung in und zwischen den Sitzungen mit der Stärke des Renewals. Jedoch waren in dieser Studie keine Unterschiede zwischen den Gruppen im Renewaltest zu beobachten, weswegen die Ergebnisse der zwei Nachtests nicht zu interpretieren sind. Das Ziel von Studie 3 (N = 61) war es, das Konzept des Kontexts im Rahmen von Konditionierung und Exposition zu definieren. In Studie 3 wurde das Auftreten der Generalisierungsabnahme (generalization decrement) genutzt, bei der eine konditionierte Reaktion infolge eines Kontextwechsels nur reduziert auftritt. Auf diesem Weg kann Kontextähnlichkeit quantifiziert werden. Nach einer Akquisitonsphase in einem Kontext wurden die Teilnehmer in einem von drei verschiedenen Kontexten getestet. Zwei dieser Kontexte unterschieden sich nur in einer Dimension (Anordnung der Objekte vs. Objekteigenschaften). Die dritte Gruppe wurde im Akquisitonskontext getestet und diente als Kontrollgruppe. Es fanden sich jedoch keine Unteschiede zwischen den Gruppen in den Testphasen. Eine mögliche Erklärung ist die Neuartigkeit des Testkontextes. Teilnehmer, die nach der Extinktion einem neuen Kontext ausgesetzt waren, erwarteten in einem anderen Kontext eine zweite Extinktionsphase und zeigten daher mehr statt weniger Angst als erwartet. N2 - Renewal of fear is one form of relapse that occurs after successful therapy, resulting from an encounter with a feared object in a context different from the context of the exposure therapy. According to Bouton (1994), the return of fear, provoked by context change, indicates that the fear was not erased in the first place. More importantly, the return of fear indicates that during the exposure session a new association was learned that connected the feared object with “no fear”; yet, as Bouton further argues, this association is context dependent. Such dependence could explain effects like renewal. In a new context, the therapeutic association will not be expressed and thus will no longer inhibit the fear. The assumption that an association is context dependent has been tested and showed robust results (Balooch & Neumann, 2011; Siavash Bandarian Balooch, Neumann, & Boschen, 2012; Culver, Stoyanova, & Craske, 2011; Kim & Richardson, 2009; Neumann & Kitlertsirivatana, 2010). Research for the treatment of anxiety disorders, aiming to reduce fear and, more importantly, prevent relapse, is flourishing. There are several exposure protocols currently under investigation: multiple contexts exposure (MCE), which aims at reducing the return of fear due to renewal (e.g., Balooch & Neumann, 2011); prolonged exposure (PE), which aims at strengthening the inhibitory association during the extinction learning (e.g., Thomas, Vurbic, & Novak, 2009); and reconsolidation update (RU), which aims at “updating” the reconsolidation process by briefly exposing the CS+ before the actual extinction takes place (Schiller et al., 2010). So far, however, few clinical studies conducted on humans have investigated these novel treatment protocols, and as far as I know none has investigated the mechanisms of action behind these protocols with a human clinical sample. The present thesis has three main goals. The first is to demonstrate that exposure therapy in multiple contexts reduces the likelihood of renewal. The second is to examine the mechanisms contributing to the effect of MCE and the third is to shed light on the concept of context in the framework of the conditioning and extinction paradigm. To this end, three studies were conducted. The first study investigated the effect of MCE on renewal, the second and third studies examined working mechanisms of MCE. In the first study thirty spider-phobic participants were exposed four times to a virtual spider. The exposure trials were conducted either in one single context or in four different contexts. Finally, all participants completed both a virtual renewal test, with the virtual spider presented in a novel virtual context, and an in vivo behavioral avoidance test with a real spider. This study successfully demonstrated the efficacy of MCE on reducing renewal. Study 2 investigated the working mechanisms behind MCE by utilizing a differential conditioning paradigm and conducting the extinction in multiple contexts, targeting similar renewal attenuation as achieved in study 1. This was followed by two tests that attempted to reveal extinction-relevant associations like ones causing context inhibitory effects. This study had three main hypotheses: (1) The extinction context is associated with the exposure, and thus operates as a safety signal at some point during the extinction; it will therefore compete with the safety learning of the CS, leading to a decreased extinction effect on the CS if the extinction is conducted in only one context. (2) The elements (e.g., room color, furniture) of the extinction context are connected to the therapeutic association and therefore should serve as reminders of the extinction, causing a stronger fear inhibition when presented during a test. (3) Therapy process factors, according to emotional processing theory, determine the renewal effect (e.g., initial fear activation, and within-session and between-session activation are correlated with the strength of renewal). In this study, however, no differences between the groups at the renewal phase were observed, presumably because the extinction was too strong to enable a renewal of fear at the test phase conducted immediately following the extinction. This hence rendered the two inhibitory tests useless. Study 3 aimed at defining the concept of context in the conditioning and exposure framework. Study 3 utilized the phenomenon known as generalization decrement, whereby a conditioned response is reduced due to change in the environment. This allowed context similarity to be quantified. After an acquisition phase in one context, participants were tested in one of three contexts, two of which differed in only one dimension (configuration of objects vs. features). The third group was tested in the same context and served as control group. The goal was to show that both configuration and features play an important role in the definition of context. There was, however, no significant statistical difference between the groups at the test phases, likely because of context novelty effects (participants exposed to a new context following extinction in another context expected a second extinction phase, and thus demonstrated greater fear than expected in all three groups). KW - Angst KW - Konditionierung KW - Virtuelle Realität KW - multiple context exposure therapy Y1 - 2013 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-76673 ER - TY - THES A1 - Walter, Martina T1 - A new methodological approach to assess drug driving – The German Smartphone Survey T1 - Ein neuer methodischer Ansatz zur Erfassung von Drogenfahrten – Die Deutsche Smartphone-Studie N2 - The aim of the present piece of work was to give information about the frequency of psychoactive substances within the German driver population and to identify preventive and promotive circumstances of drug driving. Furthermore, a new methodological approach to gather and link data about the consumption of psychoactive substances and the mobility of drug users is shown. Traditionally, roadside surveys are conducted to estimate the prevalence of drug driving within a population. By the present study, an alternative method is introduced. In total, 195 drug users (mainly cannabis users) and 100 controls out of the normal driving population were queried for four weeks about their driving and drug consumption behaviour by a questionnaire that was deployed on smartphones. The prevalences of drug driving within the sample were extrapolated into representative values. Because the subjects reported all daily activities within the study-period, it was also possible to describe situations in which the subjects decided against driving under influence. Besides, relevant previous experiences, attitudes, the approval of legal regulations, other traffic-specific parameters, social influences and personality variables were queried. So, individual factors that are associated with drug driving can be specified. The results are integrated in a model that shows dependencies of different societal, behavioural and legal variables. They can serve as major input to the discussion on drug driving and can be of practical use for rehabilitation and prevention purposes. The results can be summarised as follows: - Compared to the results of a German roadside survey from 1994, the prevalences that are found within the present study seem pretty low. This finding is discussed and possible explanations for the described trend are lined out. Furthermore, the prevalences that were calculated in the present study are compared to current data from other European countries. - The results show differences between users and controls on several variables. The differences indicate that substance use impacts on the structuring of day-to-day life. Overall, the controls’ days proceed more along a daily working routine than the users’ (e.g. less mobility at night, more mobility at rush-hour, alcohol consumption mainly at nights out). - The individual extent to which drugs are consumed differs dependent on daytime, day of the week and kind of substance. Of course, these dependencies also influence the occurrence of drug driving. Other factors of influence on drug driving are the distance, the availability of alternative modes of transport as well as the presence of female companions. - Not everybody who uses drugs drives under the influence of drugs. A striking predictor for frequent drug driving and highly intoxicated driving is a high consumption, associated with risky consumption patterns and a low subjective feeling of impairment after drug consumption. - The subjects’ attitudes towards drug driving and their beliefs about social norms largely go in line with the behaviour they engage in. Drug users have rather liberal attitudes towards drug use and driving under influence. - A possible deterrence effect of sanctioning and police enforcement and its dependence on the acceptance and awareness of the measures is delineated. - Only small effects are found when examining the objective impairment that is caused by drug use by a computer-based test battery. This result is critically discussed with regards to the operationalisation of the study groups. - Except from driving under influence, there is no evidence to suggest that DUI offenders also show problematic behaviour according to other traffic-related measures. - Parents and peers may have an influence as role models on the development of problematic behaviour. A good relationship between parents and children may have a positive impact on the development of conventional values and behaviour. - Drug use is associated with some crucial personality dimensions and drugs are often used to solve personal problems. A less precise but similar difference was found for users who commit many drives under influence compared to users who never or only sometimes drive under influence. Moreover, users marginally more often have psychological problems compared to controls. Finally, the strengths and weaknesses of the new methodological approach of data collection are discussed as well as the challenges that are faced when implementing it. All in all, it has proved to be a promising method and should serve as a standard to which future studies should aspire. N2 - Ziel der vorliegenden Arbeit war es, Informationen über das Vorkommen von Fahrten unter Substanzeinfluss in Deutschland und über Prädiktoren für das Auftreten von Drogenfahrten bereitzustellen. Des Weiteren wird ein neuer methodischer Ansatz vorgestellt, mit dem sowohl Daten über den Konsum psychoaktiver Substanzen als auch Daten über das Mobilitätsverhalten von Drogenkonsumenten erhoben und somit verknüpft werden können. Ein herkömmlicher Ansatz zur Schätzung des Vorkommens von Drogenfahrten sind Roadside Surveys. Als alternative Methode wird eine Studie beschrieben, in der 195 Drogenkonsumenten (vorwiegend Cannabiskonsumenten) sowie 100 Kontrollpersonen vier Wochen lang täglich über ein Smartphone ihr Fahr- und Konsumverhalten protokollierten. Aus der Häufigkeit von Drogenfahrten innerhalb der Stichprobe wird über eine entsprechende Gewichtung auf den tatsächlichen Anteil in der Bevölkerung geschlossen. Da von den Probanden sämtliche Aktivitäten im Erhebungszeitraum protokolliert wurden, können auch solche Situationen beschrieben werden, in denen sich die Probanden gegen eine Drogenfahrt entschieden haben. Darüber hinaus wurden Informationen über die persönliche Vorgeschichte, relevante Einstellungen, die Akzeptanz gesetzlicher Regelungen, weitere verkehrsrelevante Auffälligkeiten, soziale Einflüsse sowie Informationen über Persönlichkeitsmerkmale eingeholt. Hierdurch können Umstände und Hintergründe, unter denen Drogenfahrten stattfinden, spezifiziert werden. Die Ergebnisse werden in ein Model integriert, das Abhängigkeiten zwischen gesellschaftlichen Variablen, Verhaltensparametern und gesetzlichen Gegebenheiten aufzeigt. Die Studie liefert aufschlussreiche Befunde, die sowohl die wissenschaftliche Diskussion um Drogenfahrten anregen als auch von praktischem Nutzen für Rehabilitations- und Präventionsmaßnahmen sein können. Die Ergebnisse lassen sich wie folgt zusammenfassen: - Verglichen mit den Ergebnissen einer deutschen Roadside Survey aus dem Jahr 1994 fallen die hier kalkulierten Prävalenzen für Drogenfahrten in Deutschland recht gering aus. Der Befund wird diskutiert und es werden mögliche Gründe für den beschriebenen Trend aufgeführt. Außerdem werden die berechneten Werte mit aktuellen Vergleichsdaten aus anderen europäischen Ländern verglichen. - Der Konsum von Drogen hat gewisse Auswirkungen auf die Gestaltung des Alltags einer Person. Insgesamt scheinen Kontrollpersonen im Vergleich zu Drogenkonsumenten eher einem geregelten Arbeitsalltag nachzugehen (z. B. weniger Mobilität in der Nacht, mehr Mobilität zu Zeiten des allgemeinen Berufsverkehrs, Alkoholkonsum reduziert auf Ausgehzeiten). - Das Ausmaß des individuellen Drogenkonsums variiert in Abhängigkeit von Tageszeit, Wochentag und Art der Substanz. Natürlich wirken sich diese Abhängigkeiten ebenso auf das Auftreten von Drogenfahrten aus. Weitere situative Einflussfaktoren für Drogenfahrten sind die Länge der zurückzulegenden Strecke, die Verfügbarkeit alternativer Fortbewegungsmittel sowie weibliche Begleitpersonen. - Nicht jeder, der Drogen konsumiert, fährt auch unter Drogeneinfluss Auto. Als wesentlicher Prädiktor für häufige Drogenfahrten und hohe Substanzkonzentrationen im Blut während der Fahrt kann ein hoher Konsum genannt werden. Damit verbunden sind riskante Konsummuster sowie eine geringe subjektive Beeinträchtigung nach dem Konsum von Drogen. - Die Einstellung der Versuchspersonen zu Drogenfahrten sowie ihre Annahmen über soziale Normen stimmen zum großen Teil mit ihrem Verhalten überein. Drogenkonsumenten haben eher liberale Einstellungen zu Drogenkonsum und Drogenfahrten. - Eine mögliche abschreckende Wirkung von Sanktionen und polizeilichen Überwachungmaßnahmen und ihre Abhängigkeit von der Akzeptanz und der subjektiven Wahrnehmung der Maßnahmen wird beschrieben. - Eine objektive Leistungsbeeinträchtigung durch die Wirkung von Drogen mittels einer computerbasierten Testbatterie kann nur in geringem Maße nachgewiesen werden. Das Ergebnis wird hinsichtlich der Operationalisierung der untersuchten Gruppen kritisch diskutiert. - Es finden sich keine Hinweise dafür, dass Drogenfahrer, außer durch die Drogenfahrten an sich, auch sonst im Verkehr auffällig werden. - Eltern und Freunde scheinen als Rollenvorbilder Einfluss auf die Entwicklung von Problemverhalten zu nehmen. Eine gute Eltern-Kind-Beziehung wirkt sich positiv auf die Entwicklung konventioneller Werte und Verhalten aus. - Drogenkonsum ist mit einigen wesentlichen Persönlichkeitsmerkmalen assoziiert und Konsumenten weisen etwas häufiger psychische Probleme auf als Kontrollpersonen. Abschließend werden Stärken und Schwächen der neuen Methode diskutiert sowie besondere Anforderungen aufgeführt, die bei der Durchführung zu beachten sind. Insgesamt erwies sich die Methode als vielversprechend und sollte als Standard in künftigen Studien weiterentwickelt werden. KW - Verkehrspsychologie KW - Rauschgift KW - Alkohol KW - Fahren KW - Ambulantes Assessment KW - Ecological Momentary Assessment KW - Elektronisches Tagebuch KW - Drogen KW - Prävalenz KW - Prädiktor KW - Smartphone KW - Drugs KW - Alcohol KW - Driving KW - Prevalence KW - Predictors KW - Ambulatory Assessment KW - Ecological Momentary Assessment KW - Electronic diary KW - Mobile technology Y1 - 2012 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-75283 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Herbert, Cornelia A1 - Kübler, Andrea T1 - Dogs Cannot Bark: Event-Related Brain Responses to True and False Negated Statements as Indicators of Higher-Order Conscious Processing N2 - The present study investigated event-related brain potentials elicited by true and false negated statements to evaluate if discrimination of the truth value of negated information relies on conscious processing and requires higher-order cognitive processing in healthy subjects across different levels of stimulus complexity. The stimulus material consisted of true and false negated sentences (sentence level) and prime-target expressions (word level). Stimuli were presented acoustically and no overt behavioral response of the participants was required. Event-related brain potentials to target words preceded by true and false negated expressions were analyzed both within group and at the single subject level. Across the different processing conditions (word pairs and sentences), target words elicited a frontal negativity and a late positivity in the time window from 600–1000 msec post target word onset. Amplitudes of both brain potentials varied as a function of the truth value of the negated expressions. Results were confirmed at the single-subject level. In sum, our results support recent suggestions according to which evaluation of the truth value of a negated expression is a time- and cognitively demanding process that cannot be solved automatically, and thus requires conscious processing. Our paradigm provides insight into higher-order processing related to language comprehension and reasoning in healthy subjects. Future studies are needed to evaluate if our paradigm also proves sensitive for the detection of consciousness in non-responsive patients. KW - Psychologie Y1 - 2011 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-74907 ER - TY - THES A1 - Stemmler, Thomas T1 - Just do it! Guilt as a moral intuition to cooperate - A parallel constraint satisfaction approach T1 - Schuld als moralische Intuition zu Kooperation - ein parallel constraint satisfaction Ansatz N2 - Nach langer Dominanz rationaler Urteils- und Entscheidungsmodelle in der Moralpsychologie (z.B. Kohlberg, 1969) besteht seit einiger Zeit verstärktes Interesse an intuitiven, emotionalen Einflüssen auf moralische Urteile und Entscheidungen (z.B. Greene, 2007; Haidt, 2001; Monin, Pizarro, & Beer, 2007). Der Einfluss von Emotionen auf moralische Entscheidungen wird in der Literatur u.a. mittels heuristischer, non-kompensatorischer Informationsverarbeitung erklärt (z.B. Sinnott-Armstrong, Young, & Cushman, 2010; Sunstein, 2005; Tobler, Kalis, & Kalenscher, 2008). Hierbei wird jedoch der Prozess der Emotionsentstehung ignoriert. Appraisaltheorien postulieren, dass Emotionen durch die Inkohärenz (oder Diskrepanz) von Verhaltensrepräsentationen wie Zielen und Aktionen entstehen (Moors, 2009). Emotionsentstehung und (intuitives) Entscheiden kann in einem Modell vereint werden sobald man bei beiden Prozessen eine konnektionistische Struktur (z.B. Barnes & Thagard, 1996) zugrunde legt. Die vorliegende Arbeit kontrastiert beide Perspektiven intuitiv-emotionalen Entscheidens im Hinblick auf Schuld und Kooperation. N2 - After a long dominance of rational models of judgment and decision-making in moral psychology (e.g. Kohlberg, 1969) there is now a strong interest in how intuitions and emotions influence moral judgments and decisions (e.g. Greene, 2007; Haidt, 2001; Monin, Pizarro, & Beer, 2007). In the literature, the influence of emotions on moral decisions is explained by heuristic or non-compensatory information processing (e.g. Sinnott-Armstrong, Young, & Cushman, 2010; Sunstein, 2005; Tobler, Kalis, & Kalenscher, 2008). However, the process of emotion elicitation is ignored. Appraisal theories postulate that emotion elicitation is due to the incoherence (or discrepancy) of behavioral representations like goals and actions (Moors, 2009). Emotion elicitation and intuitive decision-making can be combined if both processes apply a connectionist information processing structure (e.g. Barnes & Thagard, 1996). The current work contrasts both perspectives of intuitive-emotional decision-making with respect to guilt and cooperation. KW - Kooperation KW - Schuldgefühl KW - Moralisches Handeln KW - parallel constraint satisfaction KW - Intuition KW - Entscheidung KW - Gefühl KW - Informationsverarbeitung KW - morality KW - guilt KW - cooperation KW - emotion KW - intuition KW - decision-making KW - parallel constraint satisfaction Y1 - 2011 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-74873 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Pfister, Roland T1 - Wardrobe Malfunctions and the Measurement of Internet Behaviour N2 - The wardrobe malfunction—an unanticipated exposure of bodily parts in the public—has become a prevailing issue in concerts, shows and other celebrity events that is reliably reported by the media. The internet as the fastest source for celebrity gossip allows measuring the impact of such wardrobe malfunctions on the public in-terest in a celebrity. This measurement in turn allows conclusions about intention, motivation, and internet be-haviour of a wide variety of internet users. The present study exemplifies the use of an innovative non-reactive measure of active interest—the Search Volume Index—to assess the impact of a variety of internet-related phe-nomena, including wardrobe malfunctions. Results indicate that interest in a celebrity increases immediately af-ter such an event and stays at a high level for about three weeks (the wardrobe plateau). This special form of ce-lebrity gossip thus meets a constant interest of a substantial proportion of internet users. KW - Psychologie KW - Internet Behaviour KW - Search Volume Index KW - Non-reactive Measurement KW - Wardrobe Malfunction Y1 - 2011 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-69067 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Münßinger, Jana I. A1 - Halder, Sebastian A1 - Kleih, Sonja C. A1 - Furdea, Adrian A1 - Raco, Valerio A1 - Hösle, Adi A1 - Kübler, Andrea T1 - Brain Painting: first evaluation of a new brain-computer interface application with ALS-patients and healthy volunteers N2 - Brain–computer interfaces (BCIs) enable paralyzed patients to communicate; however, up to date, no creative expression was possible. The current study investigated the accuracy and user-friendliness of P300-Brain Painting, a new BCI application developed to paint pictures using brain activity only. Two different versions of the P300-Brain Painting application were tested: A colored matrix tested by a group of ALS-patients (n = 3) and healthy participants (n = 10), and a black and white matrix tested by healthy participants (n = 10). The three ALS-patients achieved high accuracies; two of them reaching above 89% accuracy. In healthy subjects, a comparison between the P300-Brain Painting application (colored matrix) and the P300-Spelling application revealed significantly lower accuracy and P300 amplitudes for the P300-Brain Painting application. This drop in accuracy and P300 amplitudes was not found when comparing the P300-Spelling application to an adapted, black and white matrix of the P300-Brain Painting application. By employing a black and white matrix, the accuracy of the P300-Brain Painting application was significantly enhanced and reached the accuracy of the P300-Spelling application. ALS-patients greatly enjoyed P300-Brain Painting and were able to use the application with the same accuracy as healthy subjects. P300-Brain Painting enables paralyzed patients to express themselves creatively and to participate in the prolific society through exhibitions. KW - Psychologie KW - brain–computer interfaces KW - P300 KW - amyotrophic lateral sclerosis KW - event-related potential Y1 - 2010 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-68168 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Gerdes, Antje B. M. A1 - Wieser, Matthias J. A1 - Mühlberger, Andreas A1 - Weyers, Peter A1 - Alpers, Georg W. A1 - Plichta, Michael M. A1 - Breuer, Felix A1 - Pauli, Paul T1 - Brain activations to emotional pictures are differentially associated with valence and arousal ratings N2 - Several studies have investigated the neural responses triggered by emotional pictures, but the specificity of the involved structures such as the amygdala or the ventral striatum is still under debate. Furthermore, only few studies examined the association of stimuli’s valence and arousal and the underlying brain responses. Therefore, we investigated brain responses with functional magnetic resonance imaging of 17 healthy participants to pleasant and unpleasant affective pictures and afterwards assessed ratings of valence and arousal. As expected, unpleasant pictures strongly activated the right and left amygdala, the right hippocampus, and the medial occipital lobe, whereas pleasant pictures elicited significant activations in left occipital regions, and in parts of the medial temporal lobe. The direct comparison of unpleasant and pleasant pictures, which were comparable in arousal clearly indicated stronger amygdala activation in response to the unpleasant pictures. Most important, correlational analyses revealed on the one hand that the arousal of unpleasant pictures was significantly associated with activations in the right amygdala and the left caudate body. On the other hand, valence of pleasant pictures was significantly correlated with activations in the right caudate head, extending to the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) and the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. These findings support the notion that the amygdala is primarily involved in processing of unpleasant stimuli, particularly to more arousing unpleasant stimuli. Reward-related structures like the caudate and NAcc primarily respond to pleasant stimuli, the stronger the more positive the valence of these stimuli is. KW - Psychologie KW - emotional pictures KW - amygdala KW - caudate KW - valence KW - arousal Y1 - 2010 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-68153 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Weyers, P. A1 - Janke, W. A1 - Macht, Michael A1 - Weijers, H.-G. T1 - Social and nonsocial open field behaviour of rats under light and noise stimulation N2 - ln two experiments, male rats were observed in pairs under different environmental stimulations in an open field. ln Experiment 1, white noise of 85 dB(A) reduced social activities and increased defecation compared to 75 dB(A) and 65 dß(A). ln Experiment 2, the illumination of the open field was varied in addition to a variation of the noise intensity. Again, 85 dB(A) as compared to 50 dB(A) reduced social activities and increased defecation, but also led to changes in non-social behaviours such as sniffing, grooming, and rearing. ln contrast, 400 lx did not differ substantially in its effects from 40 lx in any of the observed behavioural categories. Altogether, the behaviour pattern under 85 dß(A) white noise cannot satisfactorily be explained only by increased anxiety or fear. Alternative explanations are discussed. KW - Psychologie KW - Social activity KW - Stress KW - Light stimulation KW - Noise stimulation KW - Environment KW - open field behaviour KW - Emotionality KW - Rat Y1 - 1993 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-61246 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Pressley, Michael A1 - Borkowski, John G. A1 - Schneider, Wolfgang T1 - Good information processing: What it is and how education can promote it N2 - The nature of good information processing is outlined as determined by intact neurology, information stored in long-term memory, and general cognitive tendencies, attitudes, and styles. Educators can promote the development of good information processing by promoting what is in long-term memory. This can be accomplished by teaching important literary, scientific, and cultural knowledge; teaching strategies; motivating the acquisition and use of important conceptual knowledge and strategies; and encouraging the general tendencies supporting good information processing. Good information processing can be produced by years of appropriate educational input. Good information processors cannot be produced by short-term interventions. KW - Psychologie Y1 - 1990 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62127 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Schneider, Wolfgang A1 - Körkel, Joachim A1 - Weinert, Franz E. T1 - Domain-Specific Knowledge and Memory Performance: A Comparison of High- and Low-Aptitude Children N2 - Two studies compared memory performance and text comprehension of groups that were equivalent on domain-specific knowledge but differed in overall aptitude, to investigate whether prior knowledge about a particular domain or overall aptitude level was more important when the task was to acquire and use new information in the domain of interest. Both studies dealt with third-, fifth-, and seventh-grade soccer experts' and novices' memory and comprehension of a story dealing with a soccer game. Several measures of memory performance, memory monitoring, and text comprehension were used. Levels of soccer knowledge and of overall aptitude were varied in a factorial design. Neither study detected significant differences between high-aptitude and low-aptitude experts, regardless of their ages. Low aptitude experts outperformed high-aptitude novices on all memory and comprehension measures. The results indicate that domain-specific knowledge can compensate for low overall aptitude on domain-related cognitive tasks. KW - Psychologie Y1 - 1989 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62107 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Kurtz-Costes, Beth E. A1 - Schneider, Wolfgang T1 - Self-concept, attributional beliefs, and school achievement: A longitudinal analysis N2 - No abstract available KW - Psychologie Y1 - 1994 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-62245 ER -