TY - THES A1 - Genheimer, Hannah T1 - The acquisition of anxiety and the impact of transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation on extinction learning in virtual contexts T1 - Angstakquisition und der Einfluß transkutaner Vagusnervstimulation auf Extinktionslernen in virtuellen Kontexten N2 - This thesis aims for a better understanding of the mechanisms underlying anxiety as well as trauma- and stressor-related disorders and the development of new therapeutic approaches. I was first interested in the associative learning mechanisms involved in the etiology of anxiety disorders. Second, I explored the therapeutic effects of transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation (tVNS) as a promising new method to accelerate and stabilize extinction learning in humans. For these purposes, I applied differential anxiety conditioning protocols realized by the implementation of virtual reality (VR). Here, a formerly neutral virtual context (anxiety context, CTX+) is presented whereby the participants unpredictably receive mildly aversive electric stimuli (unconditioned stimulus, US). Another virtual context (safety context, CTX-) is never associated with the US. Moreover, extinction of conditioned anxiety can be modeled by presenting the same contexts without US delivery. When unannounced USs were administered after extinction, i.e. reinstatement, the strength of the “returned” conditioned anxiety can provide information on the stability of the extinction memory. In Study 1, I disentangled the role of elemental and conjunctive context representations in the acquisition of conditioned anxiety. Sequential screenshots of two virtual offices were presented like a flip-book so that I elicited the impression of walking through the contexts. Some pictures of CTX+ were paired with an US (threat elements), but not some other screenshots of the same context (non-threat elements), nor the screenshots depicting CTX- (safety elements). Higher contingency ratings for threat compared to non-threat elements revealed elemental representation. Electro-cortical responses showed larger P100 and early posterior negativity amplitudes elicited by screenshots depicting CTX+ compared to CTX- and suggested conjunctive representation. These results support the dual context representation in anxiety acquisition in healthy individuals. Study 2 addressed the effects of tVNS on the stabilization of extinction learning by using a context conditioning paradigm. Potentiated startle responses as well as higher aversive ratings in CTX+ compared to CTX- indicate successful anxiety conditioning. Complete extinction was found in startle responses and valence ratings as no differentiation between CTX+ and CTX- suggested. TVNS did not affect extinction or reinstatement of anxiety which may be related to the inappropriate transferability of successful stimulation parameters from epilepsy patients to healthy participants during anxiety extinction. Therefore, in Study 3 I wanted to replicate the modulatory effects of tVNS on heart rate and pain perception by the previously used parameters. However, no effects of tVNS were observed on subjective pain ratings, on pain tolerance, or on heart rate. This led to the conclusion that the modification of stimulation parameters is necessary for a successful acceleration of anxiety extinction in humans. In Study 4, I prolonged the tVNS and, considering previous tVNS studies, I applied a cue conditioning paradigm in VR. Therefore, during acquisition a cue (CS+) presented in CTX+ predicted the US, but not another cue (CS-). Both cues were presented in a second context (CTX-) and never paired with the US. Afterward, participants received either tVNS or sham stimulation and underwent extinction learning. I found context-dependent cue conditioning only in valence ratings, which was indicated by lower valence for CS+ compared to CS- in CTX+, but no differential ratings in CTX-. Successful extinction was indicated by equal responses to CS+ and CS-. Interestingly, I found reinstatement of conditioned fear in a context-dependent manner, meaning startle response was potentiated for CS+ compared to CS- only in the anxiety context. Importantly, even the prolonged tVNS had no effect, neither on extinction nor on reinstatement of context-dependent cue conditioning. However, I found first evidence for accelerated physiological contextual extinction due to less differentiation between startles in CTX+ compared to CTX- in the tVNS than in the sham stimulated group. In sum, this thesis first confirms the dual representation of a context in an elemental and a conjunctive manner. Second, though anxiety conditioning and context-dependent cue conditioning paradigms worked well, the translation of tVNS accelerated extinction from rats to humans needs to be further developed, especially the stimulation parameters. Nevertheless, tVNS remains a very promising approach of memory enhancement, which can be particularly auspicious in clinical settings. N2 - Ziel dieser Arbeit war es, die zu Grunde liegenden Mechanismen von Angst- sowie Trauma- und belastungsbezogene Störungen besser verstehen zu lernen und neue Therapieansätze zu entwickeln. Dabei lag mein Interesse zunächst bei den assoziativen Lernmechanismen, die bei der Entstehung von Angststörungen involviert sind. Darüber hinaus untersuchte ich die therapeutischen Effekte der transkutanen Vagusnervstimulation (tVNS) als neue und vielversprechende Methode, um das Extinktionslernen bei Menschen zu beschleunigen und zu stabilisieren. Zu diesem Zweck verwendete ich differenzielle Angstkonditionierungsparadigmen in virtueller Realität (VR). Dabei wird den Probanden ein neutraler virtueller Kontext (CTX) gezeigt, in dem sie unvorhersehbare, leicht schmerzhafte elektrische Reize (unkonditionierter Stimulus, US) erhalten. Durch die erlernte Assoziation wird dieser Kontext zum (Angstkontext, CTX+). Ein zweiter virtueller Kontext, in dem die Probanden nie einen US erhalten, wird deshalb zum Sicherheitskontext (CTX-). Die Extinktion konditionierter Angst wiederum kann im Labor nachgestellt werden, indem beide Kontexte ohne US dargeboten werden. Werden aber den Probanden nach der Extinktion unangekündigte US appliziert (Reinstatement), dann kann die Stärke der zurückgekehrten Angst Aufschluss über die Stabilität des Extinktionsgedächtnisses geben. Mit diesem Modell untersuchte ich in Studie 1 die beiden Rollen der elementaren und der konjunktiven Repräsentation eines Kontexts während der Akquisition von konditionierter Angst. Nacheinander aufgenommene Bildschirmfotos zweier virtueller Büroräume wurden dabei wie in einem Daumenkino kurz hintereinander dargeboten, so dass der Eindruck entstand durch die Räume zu laufen. Der US wurde gleichzeitig mit manchen Bildern des CTX+ präsentiert (Gefahrenelemente), jedoch nie mit andere Bilder des CTX+ (keine-Gefahrenelemente) und auch nie mit Bildern, die CTX- darstellten (Sicherheitselemente). Höhere Kontingenzratings für Gefahrenelemente im Vergleich zu keine-Gefahrenelemente sprachen für die elementare Kontextrepräsentation. Elektrokortikale Signale zeigten höhere Amplituden der P100 und der frühen posterioren Negativität, die von Bildschirmfotos des CTX+ im Vergleich zum CTX- evoziert wurden, und weisen auf konjunktive Kontextrepräsentation hin. Insgesamt unterstützen diese Befunde die duale Repräsentation eines Kontexts während der Angstakquisition bei gesunden Probanden. Studie 2 thematisierte die Effekte der tVNS auf Extinktionslernen. Potenzierte Schreckreaktionen und aversivere Ratings in CTX+ verglichen mit CTX- sprachen für erfolgreiche Angstkonditionierung. Vollständige Extinktion wurde in der Schreckreaktion und in Valenzratings gefunden, da sich die Reaktionen auf CTX+ und CTX- am Ende dieser Phase nicht mehr unterschieden. Jedoch beeinflusste tVNS während der Extinktion weder das Extinktionslernen noch reduzierte sie die Wiederkehr der Angst. Aufgrund der Neuheit dieses Forschungsbereichs wurden Stimulationsparameter aus der Anwendung der tVNS bei Epilepsiepatienten gewählt. Die Übertragbarkeit auf gesunde Probanden während Angstextinktion blieb noch unklar. Deshalb sollte in Studie 3 ein tVNS Effekt auf die Herzrate und die Schmerzwahrnehmung repliziert werden, und zwar mit genau diesen Stimulationsparametern. Die Ergebnisse zeigten jedoch, dass tVNS weder subjektive Schmerzratings, noch die Schmerztoleranz, noch die Herzrate der gesunden Probanden beeinflusste. Schlussfolgernd mussten in den folgenden Untersuchungen die tVNS Parameter geändert werden, um eine erfolgreiche Wirkung der tVNS bei gesunden Probanden zu ermöglichen. In Studie 4 verlängerte ich die Stimulationszeit und adaptierte das verwendete Konditionierungsmodell zu einem Furchtkonditionierungsparadigma in VR. Dabei wurde der US während der Akquisition durch einen Hinweisreiz (CS+) im Angstkontext angekündigt, nicht jedoch durch einen zweiten Hinweisreiz (CS-). Beide Hinweisreize wurden außerdem in einem zweiten Kontext (CTX-) dargeboten und nie mit einem US gepaart. Danach unterzogen sich die gesunden Probanden entweder einer verum tVNS oder einer Scheinstimulation und durchliefen Extinktionslernen. Kontext-abhängige Furchtkonditionierung fand ich nur in Valenzratings, da die Probanden CS+ im Angstkontext negativer bewerteten als CS- und gleich im Sicherheitskontext. Erfolgreiche Extinktion zeigte sich in gleichen Bewertungen des CS+ und CS-. Interessanterweise fand ich kontext-abhängige Angstwiederkehr, d.h. Schreckreaktionen waren nur in CTX+ für CS+ potenziert im Vergleich zum CS-. Die verlängerte Stimulationszeit der tVNS hatte keinen Effekt, weder auf die Extinktion, noch auf die Wiederkehr der kontext-abhängigen konditionierten Furcht. Außerdem zeigten sich erste Tendenzen zu beschleunigter Extinktion des Kontextlernens durch tVNS, da die Schreckreaktion zwischen CTX+ und CTX- in der tVNS Gruppe weniger differenziert ausfiel als in der scheinstimulierten Gruppe. Zusammenfassend bestätigt diese Arbeit die duale Repräsentation eines Kontexts während der Angstakquisition auf neuronaler und subjektiver Ebene. Außerdem wurden erfolgreiche Angstkonditionierungs- und kontextabhängige Furchtkonditionierungs-paradigmen etabliert. Trotz keiner oder schwacher Effekte der tVNS auf Extinktion und Angstwiederkehr bleibt sie ein sehr vielversprechender Ansatz der Gedächtnissteigerung, der vor allem für den klinischen Kontext relevant ist. KW - Angst KW - Angststörung KW - anxiety conditioning KW - transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation KW - virtual reality KW - Konditionierung KW - Vagusnervstimulation Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-206390 ER - TY - THES A1 - Ahrens, Lea Marlen T1 - The Role of Attentional Control and Fear Acquisition and Generalization in Social Anxiety Disorder T1 - Die Rolle von Aufmerksamkeitskontrolle und Furchtlernen und Generalisierung bei Sozialer Angststörung N2 - Although Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD) is one of the most prevalent mental disorders, still little is known about its development and maintenance. Cognitive models assume that deviations in attentional as well as associative learning processes play a role in the etiology of SAD. Amongst others, deficits in inhibitory attentional control as well as aberrations during fear generalization, which have already been observed in other anxiety disorders, are two candidate mechanisms that might contribute to the onset and retention of SAD. However, a review of the literature shows that there is a lack of research relating to these topics. Thus, the aim of the present thesis was to examine in which way individuals with SAD differ from healthy controls regarding attentional control and generalization of acquired fear during the processing of social stimuli. Study 1 tested whether impairment in the inhibitory control of attention is a feature of SAD, and how it might be influenced by emotional expression and gaze direction of an interactional partner. For this purpose, individuals with SAD and healthy controls (HC) participated in an antisaccade task with faces displaying different emotional expressions (angry, neutral and happy) and gaze directions (direct and averted) serving as target stimuli. While the participants performed either pro- or antisaccades in response to the peripherally presented faces, their gaze behavior was recorded via eye-tracking, and ratings of valence and arousal were obtained. Results revealed that both groups showed prolonged latencies and increased error rates in trials with correct anti- compared to prosaccades. However, there were no differences between groups with regard to response latency or error rates, indicating that SAD patients did not exhibit impairment on inhibitory attentional control in comparison to HC during eye-tracking. Possible explanations for this finding could be that reduced inhibitory attentional control in SAD only occurs under certain circumstances, for example, when these individuals currently run the risk of being negatively evaluated by others and not in the mere presence of phobic stimuli, or when the cognitive load of a task is so high that it cannot be unwound by compensatory strategies, such as putting more effort into a task. As not only deviations in attentional, but also associative learning processes might be pathogenic markers of SAD, these mechanisms were further addressed in the following experiments. Study 2 is the first that attempted to investigate the generalization of conditioned fear in patients with SAD. To this end, patients with SAD and HC were conditioned to two neutral female faces serving as conditioned stimuli (CS+: reinforced; CS-: non-reinforced) and a fearful face paired with a loud scream serving as unconditioned stimulus (US). Fear generalization was tested by presenting morphs of the two faces (GS: generalization stimuli), which varied in their similarity to the original faces. During the whole experiment, self-report ratings, heart rate (HR) and skin conductance responses (SCR) were recorded. Results demonstrated that SAD patients rated all stimuli as less pleasant and more arousing, and overestimated the occurrence of the US compared to HC, indicating a general hyperarousal in individuals with SAD. In addition, ratings and SCR indicated that both groups generalized their acquired fear from the CS+ to intermediate GSs as a function of their similarity to the CS+. However, except for the HR data, which indicated that only SAD patients but not HC displayed a generalization response in this measure, most of the results did not support the hypothesis that SAD is characterized by overgeneralization. A plausible reason for this finding could be that overgeneralization is just a key characteristic of some anxiety disorders and SAD is not one of them. Still, other factors, such as comorbidities in the individuals with SAD, could also have had an influence on the results, which is why overgeneralization was further examined in study 3. The aim of study 3 was to investigate fear generalization on a neuronal level. Hence, high (HSA) and low socially anxious participants (LSA) underwent a conditioning paradigm, which was an adaption of the experimental design used study 2 for EEG. During the experiment, steady-state visually evoked potentials (ssVEPs) and ratings of valence and arousal were recorded. Analyses revealed significant generalization gradients in all ratings with highest fear responses to the CS+ and a progressive decline of these reactions with increasing similarity to the CS-. In contrast, the generalization gradient on a neuronal level showed highest amplitudes for the CS+ and a reduction in amplitude to the most proximal, but not distal GSs in the ssVEP signal, which might be interpreted as lateral inhibition in the visual cortex. The observed dissociation among explicit and implicit measures points to different functions of behavioral and sensory cortical processes during fear generalization: While the ratings might reflect an individual’s consciously increased readiness to react to threat, the lateral inhibition pattern in the occipital cortex might serve to maximize the contrast among stimuli with and without affective value and thereby improve adaptive behavior. As no group differences could be observed, the finding of study 2 that overgeneralization does not seem to be a marker of SAD is further consolidated. In sum, the conducted experiments suggest that individuals with SAD are characterized by a general hyperarousal during the exposition to disorder-relevant stimuli as indicated by enhanced arousal and reduced valence ratings of the stimuli compared to HC. However, the hypotheses that reduced inhibitory attentional control and overgeneralization of conditioned fear are markers of SAD were mostly not confirmed. Further research is required to elucidate whether they only occur under certain circumstances, such as high cognitive load (e.g. handling two tasks simultaneously) or social stress (e.g. before giving a speech), or whether they are not characteristics of SAD at all. With the help of these findings, new interventions for the treatment of SAD can be developed, such as attentional bias modification or discrimination learning. N2 - Obwohl die Soziale Angststörung (SAS) eine der häufigsten psychischen Erkrankungen ist, ist über ihre Entstehung und Aufrechterhaltung noch wenig bekannt. Kognitive Modelle nehmen an, dass Abweichungen sowohl in Aufmerksamkeits- als auch assoziativen Lernprozessen eine Rolle bei ihrer Entwicklung spielen. Unter anderem werden Defizite in der Aufmerksamkeitskontrolle sowie Abweichungen während der Generalisierung von konditionierter Furcht als für die Ätiologie potentiell bedeutsame Faktoren gehandelt, da diese Auffälligkeiten bereits bei anderen Angststörungen beobachtet wurden. Eine Literaturübersicht zeigt jedoch, dass zu dieser Thematik ein Mangel an Forschung besteht. Das Ziel der vorliegenden Doktorarbeit war es daher zu untersuchen, auf welche Weise sich Individuen mit Sozialer Angststörung bei der Verarbeitung sozialer Stimuli von gesunden Kontrollprobanden in Hinblick auf ihre Aufmerksamkeitskontrolle und die Generalisierung gelernter Furchtreaktionen unterscheiden. Studie 1 testete, ob das Vorliegen einer Beeinträchtigung der inhibitorischen Aufmerksamkeitskontrolle ein Merkmal der SAS ist, und auf welche Weise diese vom emotionalen Gesichtsausdruck sowie der Blickrichtung von Interaktionspartnern beeinflusst werden kann. Zu diesem Zweck nahmen Patienten mit SAS und eine gesunde Kontrollgruppe (KG) an einer Antisakkaden-Aufgabe teil, bei welcher Gesichter mit unterschiedlichem emotionalen Ausdruck (wütend, neutral und fröhlich) und unterschiedlicher Blickrichtung (direkter und abgewandter Blick) als Stimuli dienten. Während die Probanden in Abhängigkeit eines Hinweisreizes entweder Pro- oder Antisakkaden in Reaktion auf die peripher präsentierten Gesichter ausübten, wurde ihr Blickverhalten mittels Eye-Tracking aufgezeichnet. Außerdem wurden anschließend Valenz- und Arousal-Ratings der Stimuli erfasst. Die Ergebnisse zeigten, dass beide Gruppen erhöhte Latenzzeiten sowie Fehlerraten in Durchgängen mit korrekt ausgeführten Antisakkaden im Vergleich zu Prosakkaden aufwiesen. Jedoch gab es keinen Gruppenunterschied in Bezug auf die Antwortlatenz und Fehlerrate, was darauf hindeutet, dass Patienten mit SAS im Vergleich zur KG kein Defizit der inhibitorischen Aufmerksamkeitskontrolle während des Eye-Trackings erkennen ließen. Eine mögliche Ursache für diesen Befund könnte sein, dass eine reduzierte inhibitorische Aufmerksamkeitskontrolle bei SAS nur unter bestimmten Umständen auftritt, beispielsweise, wenn betroffene Individuen akut Gefahr laufen von anderen negativ bewertet zu werden, und nicht bloß phobischen Stimuli ausgesetzt sind, oder wenn die kognitive Belastung durch eine Aufgabe so groß ist, dass sie nicht durch kompensatorische Strategien, wie beispielsweise mehr Anstrengung, ausgeglichen werden kann. Da nicht nur abweichende Aufmerksamkeitsprozesse, sondern auch abweichende assoziative Lernprozesse pathogene Marker von SAS sein könnten, wurden letztere in den folgenden Experimenten genauer untersucht. Studie 2 stellt den ersten Versuch dar die Generalisierung konditionierter Furcht in Patienten mit SAS zu erforschen. Hierfür wurden sowohl SAS Patienten als auch eine KG auf zwei neutrale, weibliche Gesichter konditioniert, welche als Konditionierungsstimuli (conditioned stimuli [CS]: CS+: verstärkt; CS-: unverstärkt) dienten. Bei dem unkonditionierten Stimulus (unconditioned stimulus [US]) handelte es sich um die bereits bekannten Gesichter mit ängstlichem Ausdruck, die mit einem lauten Schrei gepaart wurden. Die Furchtgeneralisierung wurde mittels der Präsentation von Gesichtern, welche aus den beiden Ursprungsgesichtern gemorpht worden waren und als Generalisierungsstimuli (generalization stimuli [GS]) dienten, getestet. Während des Experiments wurden Selbstauskunftsratings sowie Herzrate (heart rate [HR]) und Hautleitfähigkeit (skin conductance response [SCR]) aufgezeichnet. Die Ergebnisse zeigten, dass Patienten mit SAS im Vergleich zur KG alle Stimuli als unangenehmer und aufregender bewerteten sowie die Auftretenswahrscheinlichkeit des US überschätzten, was auf eine generelle Übererregung in Individuen mit SAS hinweist. Darüber hinaus ergaben die Ergebnisse, dass beide Gruppen ihre erworbene Furcht vom CS+ in Abhängigkeit ihrer Ähnlichkeit mit dem CS+ auf intermediäre GSs übertrugen. Allerdings stützen abgesehen von den Daten der Herzrate, in denen nur SAS Patienten und nicht die KG eine Generalisierungsreaktion zeigten, die meisten Befunde nicht die Hypothese, dass Übergeneralisierung ein Merkmal von SAS ist. Eine mögliche Ursache dieses Ergebnisses könnte sein, dass Übergeneralisierung nur ein wichtiges Merkmal einiger bestimmter Angststörungen ist und SAS nicht zu ihnen gehört. Dennoch könnten auch andere Faktoren, wie beispielsweise die Komorbiditäten der untersuchten SAS Patienten, einen Einfluss auf die Ergebnisse gehabt haben. Aus diesem Grund wurde Übergeneralisierung in Studie 3 näher untersucht. Das Ziel von Studie 3 war es Furchtgeneralisierung auf neuronaler Ebene zu untersuchen. Folglich wurde das Paradigma der zweiten Studie an einen Versuchsplan, der für die Messung von neuronaler Aktivität mittels EEG geeignet war, angepasst und auf eine hoch (high socially anxious [HSA])- sowie eine niedrig sozialängstliche Gruppe (low socially anxious [LSA]) angewandt. Während des Experiments wurden sowohl steady-state visually evoked potentials (ssVEPs) als auch Valenz- und Arousal-Ratings erfasst. Die Analyse ergab signifikante Generalisierungsgradienten in allen Ratings mit der höchsten Furchtreaktion auf den CS+ und einem fortschreitenden Abfall der Reaktion auf die GSs mit zunehmender Ähnlichkeit zum CS-. Im Gegensatz dazu zeigte sich in der ssVEP-Amplitude ein anderes Muster: hier erreichte der Generalisierungsgradient zwar auch die höchste Amplitude in Reaktion auf den CS+, jedoch eine anschließende Reduktion der Amplitude auf den nächst proximalen, nicht jedoch distale GS, was ein Hinweis auf laterale Hemmungsprozesse im visuellen Kortex sein könnte. Die beobachtete Dissoziation zwischen expliziten und impliziten Maßen könnte auf unterschiedliche Funktionen von behavioralen und sensorischen kortikalen Prozessen während der Generalisierung von Furcht hinweisen: Während die Ratings möglicherweise die bewusste Bereitschaft eines Individuums auf Bedrohung zu reagieren widerspiegeln, könnte das Muster lateraler Hemmung im okzipitalen Kortex dazu dienen den Kontrast zwischen Stimuli mit und ohne affektivem Wert zu maximieren und somit adaptives Verhalten verbessern. Da zwischen beiden Gruppen keine signifikanten Unterschiede gefunden wurden, untermauerte Studie 3 das Ergebnis von Studie 2, welches bereits eher dagegen sprach, dass Übergeneralisierung von Furcht ein Merkmal von Individuen mit SAS sei. Insgesamt suggerieren die Ergebnisse der durchgeführten Studien, dass Individuen mit SAS während der Exposition von störungsspezifischen Reizen im Vergleich zu Kontrollprobanden durch eine generelle Übererregung gekennzeichnet sind, was an erhöhten Arousal- und verringerten Valenz-Ratings erkennbar war. Jedoch konnten die Hypothesen, dass reduzierte Aufmerksamkeitskontrolle sowie Übergeneralisierung Merkmale von Individuen mit SAS sind, zum größten Teil nicht bestätigt werden. Weitere Forschung ist nötig um herauszufinden, ob diese Phänomene nur unter besonderen äußeren Umständen, wie beispielsweise hohen kognitiven Anforderungen (e.g. bei der Bearbeitung zweier Aufgaben gleichzeitig) oder sozialem Stress (e.g. vor dem Halten einer Rede), auftreten, oder ob sie gar kein Merkmal von SAS darstellen. Mit Hilfe der sich daraus ergebenden Befunde könnten neue Interventionen für die Behandlung von SAS entwickelt werden, wie beispielsweise Aufmerksamkeitsbias-Modifikations-Trainings oder Diskriminationslernen. KW - Sozialangst KW - Aufmerksamkeit KW - social anxiety KW - fear generalization KW - visual attention KW - Psychologie KW - Visuelle Aufmerksamkeit KW - Aversive Konditionierung Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-171622 ER - TY - THES A1 - Lange [né Söhnchen], Bastian T1 - Influence of social anxiety on social attention and corresponding changes in action patterns T1 - Einfluss der sozialen Angst auf die soziale Aufmerksamkeit und korrespondierende Veränderungen im Bewegungsverhalten N2 - People who suffer Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD) are under substantial personal distress and endure impaired normal functioning in at least some parts of everyday life. Next, to the personal suffering, there are also the immense public health costs to consider, as SAD is the most common anxiety disorder and thereby one of the major psychiatric disorders in general. Over the last years, fundamental research found cognitive factors as essential components in the development and maintenance of social fears. Following leading cognitive models, avoidance behaviors are thought to be an important factor in maintaining the developed social anxieties. Therefore, this thesis aims to deepen the knowledge of avoidance behaviors exhibited in social anxiety, which allows to get a better understanding of how SAD is maintained. To reach this goal three studies were conducted, each using a different research approach. In the first study cutting-edge Virtual Reality (VR) equipment was used to immerse participants in a virtual environment. In this virtual setting, High Socially Anxious (HSA) individuals and matched controls had to execute a social Approach-Avoidance Task (AAT). In the task, participants had to pass a virtual person displaying neutral or angry facial expressions. By using a highly immersive VR apparatus, the first described study took the initial step in establishing a new VR task for the implicit research on social approach-avoidance behaviors. By moving freely through a VR environment, participants experienced near real-life social situations. By tracking body and head movements, physical and attentional approach-avoidance processes were studied. The second study looked at differences in attention shifts initiated by gaze-cues of neutral or emotional faces. Comparing HSA and controls, enabled a closer look at attention re-allocation with special focus on social stimuli. Further, context conditioning was used to compare task performance in a safe and in a threatening environment. Next to behavioral performance, the study also investigated neural activity using Electroencephalography (EEG) primarily looking at the N2pc component. In the third study, eye movements of HSA and Low Socially Anxious (LSA) were analyzed using an eye-tracking apparatus while participants executed a computer task. The participants’ tasks consisted of the detection of either social or non-social stimuli in complex visual settings. The study intended to compare attention shifts towards social components between these two tasks and how high levels of social anxiety influence them. In other words, the measurements of eye movements enabled the investigation to what extent social attention is task-dependent and how it is influenced by social anxiety. With the three described studies, three different approaches were used to get an in-depth understanding of what avoidance behaviors in SAD are and to which extent they are exhibited. Overall, the results showed that HSA individuals exhibited exaggerated physical and attentional avoidance behavior. Furthermore, the results highlighted that the task profoundly influences attention allocation. Finally, all evidence indicates that avoidance behaviors in SAD are exceedingly complex. They are not merely based on the fear of a particular stimulus, but rather involve highly compound cognitive processes, which surpass the simple avoidance of threatening stimuli. To conclude, it is essential that further research is conducted with special focus on SAD, its maintaining factors, and the influence of the chosen research task and method. N2 - Menschen, die unter einer sozialen Angststörung leiden, stehen unter erheblicher persönlicher Belastung und leiden teilweise unter Beeinträchtigung der normalen Funktionsfähigkeit. Neben den persönlichen Belastungen sind auch die immensen Kosten für das Gesundheitswesen zu berücksichtigen, da die soziale Angststörung eine der häufigsten psychiatrischen Störungen ist. Die Grundlagenforschung hat kognitive Faktoren als wesentliche Komponenten bei der Entwicklung und Aufrechterhaltung sozialer Ängste identifiziert. Nach führenden kognitiven Modellen wird angenommen, dass Vermeidungsverhalten ein wichtiger Faktor für die Aufrechterhaltung der entwickelten sozialen Ängste ist. Die vorliegende Arbeit hatte als Ziel, einen tieferen Einblick in das Vermeidungs- verhalten von sozial Ängstlichen zu bekommen, um ein umfangreicheres Verständnis für die Aufrechterhaltung von sozialen Angststörungen zu bekommen. Um dieses Ziel zu erreichen, wurden drei Studien durchgeführt. In der ersten Studie wurde modernstes Virtual Reality (VR) Equipment eingesetzt, um die Versuchsteilnehmer in eine virtuelle Umgebung eintauchen zu lassen. In dieser virtuellen Realität mussten Hoch-Sozialängstliche (HSÄ) und Kontrollprobanden einen sozialen Approach-Avoidance Task (AAT) durchführen. In dieser Aufgabe mussten die Teilnehmer eine virtuelle Person passieren, welche einen neutralen oder wüten- den Gesichtsausdruck zeigte. Mit der Verwendung eines hochgradig immersiven VR Systems, unternahm die Studie den ersten Schritt zur Etablierung einer neuartigen VR Aufgabe für die implizite Erforschung des Verhaltens bei sozialer Vermeidung. Mithilfe von nahezu unbeschränkter Bewegung, durch eine virtuelle Umgebung, er- lebten die Teilnehmer realitätsnahe soziale Situationen. Die zweite Studie untersuchte Unterschiede in der Aufmerksamkeitsverschiebung, die durch die Beobachtung von neutralen oder emotionalen Gesichtern aus- gelöst wurde. Hierbei wurde das Verhalten von HSÄ und Niedrig-Sozialängstliche (NSÄ) verglichen, um den Einfluss von sozialer Ängstlichkeit bei Neuzuweisung von Aufmerksamkeit in Bezug auf soziale Reize zu messen. Zusätzlich wurde Kontextkonditionierung verwendet, um die Aufmerksamkeitsverschiebung in einer sicheren und einer bedrohlichen Umgebung zu vergleichen. Neben dem Aufmerksamkeitsverhalten untersuchte die Studie auch die neuronale Aktivität mittels Electroencephalography (EEG), wobei vor allem die N2pc-Komponente untersucht wurde. In der dritten Studie wurden die Augenbewegungen von HSÄ und NSÄ ana- lysiert, während die Teilnehmer eine Computeraufgabe durchführten. Zu den Auf- gaben, gehörte das Erkennen von sozialen oder nicht-sozialen Reizen in komplexen visuellen Darstellungen. Ziel der Studie war es, Aufmerksamkeitsverschiebungen in Richtung sozialer Komponenten zwischen den beiden Aufgaben zu vergleichen. Darüber hinaus wurde untersucht, welchen Einfluss die soziale Angst auf diesen Prozess hat. Mit anderen Worten, die Messungen der Augenbewegungen ermöglichte zu untersuchen, inwieweit die soziale Aufmerksamkeit aufgabenabhängig ist und wie diese Abhängigkeit von sozialer Angst beeinflusst wird. Mit den drei Studien wurden drei unterschiedliche Ansätze verwendet, um besser zu verstehen, welches Vermeidungsverhalten Individuen mit sozialer Angststörung ausführen und wie ausgeprägt dieses ist. Insgesamt zeigen die Ergebnisse, dass HSÄ eine verstärkte Vermeidung im Verhalten und Aufmerksamkeit aufweisen. Darüber hinaus zeigen die Resultate, dass die instruierte Aufgabe einen wesentlichen Einfluss auf die Aufmerksamkeitsverteilung hat. Zusammenfassend deuten alle Ergebnisse darauf hin, dass das Vermeidungsverhalten in der sozialen Angststörung sehr komplex ist. Dieses basiert hierbei nicht einfach nur auf der Angst vor einem bestimmten Reiz, sondern beinhaltet hochkomplexe kognitive Prozesse, die über eine einfache Flucht-Reaktionen hinausgehen. Abschließend ist es unerlässlich, dass weitere Forschungen über die soziale Angststörung, ihre Erhaltungsfaktoren und den Einfluss der gewählten Forschungsaufgabe und -methode durchgeführt werden. KW - Sozialangst KW - Virtuelle Realität KW - Elektroencephalographie KW - Augenbewegung KW - Vermeidungsreaktion KW - Kontextkonditionierung KW - Virtuelle Realität KW - Elektroencephalographie KW - Augenbewegung KW - Vermeidungsverhalten Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-189001 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Hoffmann, Mareike A. A1 - Pieczykolan, Aleks A1 - Koch, Iring A1 - Huestegge, Lynn T1 - Two sources of task prioritization: The interplay of effector-based and task order-based capacity allocation in the PRP paradigm JF - Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics N2 - When processing of two tasks overlaps, performance is known to suffer. In the well-established psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm, tasks are triggered by two stimuli with a short temporal delay (stimulus onset asynchrony; SOA), thereby allowing control of the degree of task overlap. A decrease of the SOA reliably yields longer RTs of the task associated with the second stimulus (Task 2) while performance in the other task (Task 1) remains largely unaffected. This Task 2-specific SOA effect is usually interpreted in terms of central capacity limitations. Particularly, it has been assumed that response selection in Task 2 is delayed due to the allocation of less capacity until this process has been completed in Task 1. Recently, another important factor determining task prioritization has been proposed—namely, the particular effector systems associated with tasks. Here, we study both sources of task prioritization simultaneously by systematically combining three different effector systems (pairwise combinations of oculomotor, vocal, and manual responses) in the PRP paradigm. Specifically, we asked whether task order-based task prioritization (SOA effect) is modulated as a function of Task 2 effector system. The results indicate a modulation of SOA effects when the same (oculomotor) Task 1 is combined with a vocal versus a manual Task 2. This is incompatible with the assumption that SOA effects are solely determined by Task 1 response selection duration. Instead, they support the view that dual-task processing bottlenecks are resolved by establishing a capacity allocation scheme fed by multiple input factors, including attentional weights associated with particular effector systems. KW - cognitive and attentional control KW - dual-task performance KW - dual task procedures (PRP) Introduction In everyday Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-235365 SN - 1943-3921 VL - 82 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Seitz, Maximilian A1 - Lenhart, Jan A1 - Rübsam, Nina T1 - The effects of gendered information in stories on preschool children’s development of gender stereotypes JF - British Journal of Developmental Psychology N2 - Social‐cognitive theory posits that children learn gender stereotypes through gendered information. The present study examined whether children learn new gender stereotypes from stories when unknown words are linked to a gendered protagonist or context information. In Experiment 1, 40 3‐ to 6‐year‐old preschoolers were read stories with either a gendered protagonist embedded within a non‐gendered context, or a non‐gendered protagonist embedded within a gendered context. In Experiment 2, the same sample of children were read stories with the protagonist and the context displaying congruent or incongruent gender information. Each story featured an unknown activity linked with the stereotypical content. Both experiments indicate that the children rated the activity according to both the gender of the context and of the protagonist; however, the effect of the latter was stronger. In addition, children showed higher interest in the unknown activity if the protagonist’s gender matched their own sex. Thus, gender information in stories influences how children perceive unknown words. KW - gender development KW - gender stereotypes KW - shared‐reading KW - social‐cognitive theory KW - storybooks Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-214622 VL - 38 IS - 3 SP - 363 EP - 390 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Kaussner, Y. A1 - Kuraszkiewicz, A. M. A1 - Schoch, S. A1 - Markel, Petra A1 - Hoffmann, S. A1 - Baur-Streubel, R. A1 - Kenntner-Mabiala, R. A1 - Pauli, P. T1 - Treating patients with driving phobia by virtual reality exposure therapy – a pilot study JF - PLoS ONE N2 - Objectives Virtual reality exposure therapy (VRET) is a promising treatment for patients with fear of driving. The present pilot study is the first one focusing on behavioral effects of VRET on patients with fear of driving as measured by a post-treatment driving test in real traffic. Methods The therapy followed a standardized manual including psychotherapeutic and medical examination, two preparative psychotherapy sessions, five virtual reality exposure sessions, a final behavioral avoidance test (BAT) in real traffic, a closing session, and two follow-up phone assessments after six and twelve weeks. VRE was conducted in a driving simulator with a fully equipped mockup. The exposure scenarios were individually tailored to the patients’ anxiety hierarchy. A total of 14 patients were treated. Parameters on the verbal, behavioral and physiological level were assessed. Results The treatment was helpful to overcome driving fear and avoidance. In the final BAT, all patients mastered driving tasks they had avoided before, 71% showed an adequate driving behavior as assessed by the driving instructor, and 93% could maintain their treatment success until the second follow-up phone call. Further analyses suggest that treatment reduces avoidance behavior as well as symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder as measured by standardized questionnaires (Avoidance and Fusion Questionnaire: p < .10, PTSD Symptom Scale–Self Report: p < .05). Conclusions VRET in driving simulation is very promising to treat driving fear. Further research with randomized controlled trials is needed to verify efficacy. Moreover, simulators with lower configuration stages should be tested for a broad availability in psychotherapy. KW - Mental health therapies KW - Heart rate KW - Animal behavior KW - Instructors KW - Psychometrics KW - Post-traumatic stress disorder KW - Fear KW - Pilot studies Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-201051 VL - 15 IS - 1 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Ingerslev, Line Ryberg T1 - Inhibited Intentionality: On Possible Self-Understanding in Cases of Weak Agency JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - The paper addresses the question of how to approach consciousness in unreflective actions. Unreflective actions differ from reflective, conscious actions in that the intentional description under which the agent knows what she is doing is not available or present to the agent at the moment of acting. Yet, unreflective actions belong to the field in which an agent experiences herself as capable of acting. Some unreflective actions, however, narrow this field and can be characterized by intentionality being inhibited. By studying inhibited intentionality in unreflective actions, the aim of the paper is to show how weaker forms of action urge us to expand our overall understanding of action. If we expand the field of actions such that it encompasses also some of the involuntary aspects of action, we are able to understand how unreflective actions can remain actions and do not fall under the scope of automatic behavior. With the notion of weak agency, the paper thus addresses one aspect of unreflective action, namely, “inhibited intentionality” in which an agent feels a diminished sense of authorship in relation to her possibility for self-understanding. The notion of weak agency clarifies how agency itself remains intact but can involve a process of appropriation of one’s actions as one’s own. With a diachronic account of consciousness in unreflective action, the paper accounts for possible self-understanding in cases where none seems available at the moment of action. KW - unreflective actions KW - habits KW - consciousness KW - action KW - responsibility KW - diachronicity Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-212950 SN - 1664-1078 VL - 11 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Poetzsch, Tristan A1 - Germanakos, Panagiotis A1 - Huestegge, Lynn T1 - Toward a Taxonomy for Adaptive Data Visualization in Analytics Applications JF - Frontiers in Artificial Intelligence N2 - Data analytics as a field is currently at a crucial point in its development, as a commoditization takes place in the context of increasing amounts of data, more user diversity, and automated analysis solutions, the latter potentially eliminating the need for expert analysts. A central hypothesis of the present paper is that data visualizations should be adapted to both the user and the context. This idea was initially addressed in Study 1, which demonstrated substantial interindividual variability among a group of experts when freely choosing an option to visualize data sets. To lay the theoretical groundwork for a systematic, taxonomic approach, a user model combining user traits, states, strategies, and actions was proposed and further evaluated empirically in Studies 2 and 3. The results implied that for adapting to user traits, statistical expertise is a relevant dimension that should be considered. Additionally, for adapting to user states different user intentions such as monitoring and analysis should be accounted for. These results were used to develop a taxonomy which adapts visualization recommendations to these (and other) factors. A preliminary attempt to validate the taxonomy in Study 4 tested its visualization recommendations with a group of experts. While the corresponding results were somewhat ambiguous overall, some aspects nevertheless supported the claim that a user-adaptive data visualization approach based on the principles outlined in the taxonomy can indeed be useful. While the present approach to user adaptivity is still in its infancy and should be extended (e.g., by testing more participants), the general approach appears to be very promising. KW - graph adaptivity KW - data visualization KW - user model KW - analytics KW - graph ergonomics KW - recommendation engine Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-202074 SN - 2624-8212 VL - 3 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Liesner, Marvin A1 - Kirsch, Wladimir A1 - Pfister, Roland A1 - Kunde, Wilfried T1 - Spatial action-effect binding depends on type of action-effect transformation JF - Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics N2 - Spatial action–effect binding denotes the mutual attraction between the perceived position of an effector (e.g., one’s own hand) and a distal object that is controlled by this effector. Such spatial binding can be construed as an implicit measure of object ownership, thus the belonging of a controlled object to the own body. The current study investigated how different transformations of hand movements (body-internal action component) into movements of a visual object (body-external action component) affect spatial action–effect binding, and thus implicit object ownership. In brief, participants had to bring a cursor on the computer screen into a predefined target position by moving their occluded hand on a tablet and had to estimate their final hand position. In Experiment 1, we found a significantly lower drift of the proprioceptive position of the hand towards the visual object when hand movements were transformed into laterally inverted cursor movements, rather than cursor movements in the same direction. Experiment 2 showed that this reduction reflected an elimination of spatial action–effect binding in the inverted condition. The results are discussed with respect to the prerequisites for an experience of ownership over artificial, noncorporeal objects. Our results show that predictability of an object movement alone is not a sufficient condition for ownership because, depending on the type of transformation, integration of the effector and a distal object can be fully abolished even under conditions of full controllability. KW - action–effect compatibility KW - agency KW - body ownership KW - ideomotor theory KW - proprioceptive drift KW - spatial binding KW - tool use Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-232781 SN - 1943-3921 VL - 82 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Endlich, Darius A1 - Richter, Tobias A1 - Marx, Peter A1 - Lenhard, Wolfgang A1 - Moll, Kristina A1 - Witzel, Björn A1 - Schulte-Körne, Gerd T1 - Spelling Error Detection : A Valid and Economical Task for Assessing Spelling Skills in Elementary-School Children JF - Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie N2 - The ability to spell words correctly is a key competence for educational and professional achievement. Economical procedures are essential to identifying children with spelling problems as early as possible. Given the strong evidence showing that reading and spelling are based on the same orthographic knowledge, error-detection tasks (EDTs) could be considered such an economical procedure. Although EDTs are widely used in English-speaking countries, the few studies in German-speaking countries investigated only pupils in secondary school. The present study investigated N = 1,513 children in elementary school. We predicted spelling competencies (measured by dictation or gap-fill dictation) based on an EDT via linear regression. Error-detection abilities significantly predicted spelling competencies (R² between .509 and .679), indicating a strong connection. Predictive values in identifying children with poor spelling abilities with an EDT proved to be sufficient. Error detection for the assessment of spelling skills is therefore a valid instrument for transparent languages as well. N2 - Rechtschreibung zählt zu den Schlüsselkompetenzen für schulischen und beruflichen Erfolg. Um Kinder mit Rechtschreibproblemen adäquat zu unterstützen, ist eine frühe, möglichst niederschwellige Diagnostik essenziell. Aufgaben, in denen Rechtschreibfehler in präsentierten Texten zu identifizieren sind, könnten derartige ökonomische Verfahren darstellen. Obgleich Fehleridentifikationstests im angloamerikanischen Sprachraum weit verbreitet sind, haben sich die wenigen Studien im deutschsprachigen Raum bisher ausschließlich mit Kindern der Sekundarstufe beschäftigt. Die vorliegende Arbeit untersuchte in vier unabhängigen Studien N = 1.513 Grundschulkinder. Mittels linearer Regressionen wurden Rechtschreibkompetenzen (erhoben durch Fließ- und Lückendiktate) durch Leistungen in Fehleridentifikationstests vorhergesagt. Leistungen im Fehleridentifikationstest sagten Rechtschreibkompetenzen in allen Studien signifikant voraus (R² zwischen .509 und .679), was eine starke Assoziation der beiden Maße belegt. Prädiktive Werte zur Identifikation von Kindern mit schwachen Rechtschreibleistungen durch den Fehleridentifikationstest waren gut. Fehleridentifikation als Maß für Rechtschreibkompetenzen ist damit ein valides Instrument nicht nur für den angloamerikanischen Sprachraum, sondern auch für transparente Sprachen. T2 - Fehleridentifikation: Ein valides und ökonomisches Verfahren zur Erfassung von Rechtschreibkompetenzen in der Grundschule KW - spelling KW - dictation KW - error detection KW - developmental dyslexia KW - diagnosis KW - Rechtschreibung KW - Diktat KW - Fehleridentifikation KW - Lese-Rechtschreibstörung KW - Diagnose Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-244665 SN - 0049-8637 SN - 2190-6262 VL - 52 IS - 1-2 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Cao, Liyu A1 - Chen, Xinyu A1 - Haendel, Barbara F. T1 - Overground Walking Decreases Alpha Activity and Entrains Eye Movements in Humans JF - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience N2 - Experiments in animal models have shown that running increases neuronal activity in early visual areas in light as well as in darkness. This suggests that visual processing is influenced by locomotion independent of visual input. Combining mobile electroencephalography, motion- and eye-tracking, we investigated the influence of overground free walking on cortical alpha activity (~10 Hz) and eye movements in healthy humans. Alpha activity has been considered a valuable marker of inhibition of sensory processing and shown to negatively correlate with neuronal firing rates. We found that walking led to a decrease in alpha activity over occipital cortex compared to standing. This decrease was present during walking in darkness as well as during light. Importantly, eye movements could not explain the change in alpha activity. Nevertheless, we found that walking and eye related movements were linked. While the blink rate increased with increasing walking speed independent of light or darkness, saccade rate was only significantly linked to walking speed in the light. Pupil size, on the other hand, was larger during darkness than during light, but only showed a modulation by walking in darkness. Analyzing the effect of walking with respect to the stride cycle, we further found that blinks and saccades preferentially occurred during the double support phase of walking. Alpha power, as shown previously, was lower during the swing phase than during the double support phase. We however could exclude the possibility that the alpha modulation was introduced by a walking movement induced change in electrode impedance. Overall, our work indicates that the human visual system is influenced by the current locomotion state of the body. This influence affects eye movement pattern as well as neuronal activity in sensory areas and might form part of an implicit strategy to optimally extract sensory information during locomotion. KW - mobile EEG KW - alpha oscillations KW - blinks KW - saccades KW - locomotion KW - pupil size KW - walking phase KW - motor entrainment Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-219872 SN - 1662-5161 VL - 14 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Wertgen, Andreas G. A1 - Richter, Tobias T1 - Source credibility modulates the validation of implausible information JF - Memory & Cognition N2 - Validation of text information as a general mechanism for detecting inconsistent or false information is an integral part of text comprehension. This study examined how the credibility of the information source affects validation processes. Two experiments investigated combined effects of source credibility and plausibility of information during validation with explicit (ratings) and implicit (reading times) measurements. Participants read short stories with a high-credible versus low-credible person that stated a consistent or inconsistent assertion with general world knowledge. Ratings of plausibility and ratings of source credibility were lower when a credible source stated a world-knowledge inconsistent assertion compared with a low-credible source. Reading times on target sentences and on spillover sentences were slower when a credible source stated an assertion inconsistent with world knowledge compared with a low-credible source, suggesting that source information modulated the validation of implausible information. These results show that source credibility modulates validation and suggest a bidirectional relationship of perceived plausibility and source credibility in the reading process. KW - validation KW - plausibility KW - sourcing KW - credibility KW - text comprehension Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-234825 SN - 0090-502X VL - 48 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Gesell, Nathalie A1 - Niklas, Frank A1 - Schmiedeler, Sandra A1 - Segerer, Robin T1 - Mindfulness and romantic relationship outcomes: the mediating role of conflict resolution styles and closeness JF - Mindfulness N2 - Objectives The association of mindfulness and romantic relationship outcomes such as partnership quality and satisfaction is well-established; however, the mechanisms of action are not yet clear. The current study tested conflict resolution styles and closeness as possible mediating factors. We hypothesized that trait mindfulness would increase the use of constructive conflict resolution styles (positive problem solving), decrease the use of destructive styles (conflict engagement, withdrawal, and compliance), and promote feelings of closeness between partners, which in turn would predict positive relationship outcomes (namely partnership quality, partnership satisfaction, and sexual satisfaction). Methods A total of 209 individuals (86% German, 76% female, mean age = 32 years) living in a relationship (31% married) participated in an online questionnaire. Results Mediation analyses revealed that positive problem solving mediated the association between mindfulness and partnership quality with b = .09 (95% CI = .03–.17), mindfulness and partnership satisfaction with b = .07 (95% CI = .02–.13), and mindfulness and sexual satisfaction with b = .04 (95% CI = .00–.10). Furthermore, a mediating role of withdrawal and closeness was shown for individual relationship outcomes. Conclusions Findings suggest that more positive problem solving, less withdrawal, and more closeness are mechanisms by which mindfulness is associated with positive relationship outcomes. The results of our study thus broaden our understanding of the processes that underlie fulfilling romantic relationships and, in turn, underline the positive effects of mindfulness. KW - mindfulness KW - trait mindfulness KW - romantic relationship KW - relationship satisfaction KW - closeness KW - conflict resolution Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-235080 SN - 1868-8527 VL - 11 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Eder, Andreas B. A1 - Mitschke, Vanessa A1 - Gollwitzer, Mario T1 - What stops revenge taking? Effects of observed emotional reactions on revenge seeking JF - Aggressive Behavior N2 - What reaction stops revenge taking? Four experiments (total N = 191) examined this question where the victim of an interpersonal transgression could observe the offender's reaction (anger, sadness, pain, or calm) to a retributive noise punishment. We compared the punishment intensity selected by the participant before and after seeing the offender's reaction. Seeing the opponent in pain reduced subsequent punishment most strongly, while displays of sadness and verbal indications of suffering had no appeasing effect. Expression of anger about a retributive punishment did not increase revenge seeking relative to a calm reaction, even when the anger response was disambiguated as being angry with the punisher. It is concluded that the expression of pain is the most effective emotional display for the reduction of retaliatory aggression. The findings are discussed in light of recent research on reactive aggression and retributive justice. KW - aggression KW - emotion display KW - retaliation Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-214686 VL - 46 IS - 4 SP - 305 EP - 316 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Haspert, Valentina A1 - Wieser, Matthias J. A1 - Pauli, Paul A1 - Reicherts, Philipp T1 - Acceptance-Based Emotion Regulation Reduces Subjective and Physiological Pain Responses JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - Acceptance-based regulation of pain, which focuses on the allowing of pain and pain related thoughts and emotions, was found to modulate pain. However, results so far are inconsistent regarding different pain modalities and indices. Moreover, studies so far often lack a suitable control condition, focus on behavioral pain measures rather than physiological correlates, and often use between-subject designs, which potentially impede the evaluation of the effectiveness of the strategies. Therefore, we investigated whether acceptance-based strategies can reduce subjective and physiological markers of acute pain in comparison to a control condition in a within-subject design. To this end, participants (N = 30) completed 24 trials comprising 10 s of heat pain stimulation. Each trial started with a cue instructing participants to welcome and experience pain (acceptance trials) or to react to the pain as it is without employing any regulation strategies (control trials). In addition to pain intensity and unpleasantness ratings, heart rate (HR) and skin conductance (SC) were recorded. Results showed significantly decreased pain intensity and unpleasantness ratings for acceptance compared to control trials. Additionally, HR was significantly lower during acceptance compared to control trials, whereas SC revealed no significant differences. These results demonstrate the effectiveness of acceptance-based strategies in reducing subjective and physiological pain responses relative to a control condition, even after short training. Therefore, the systematic investigation of acceptance in different pain modalities in healthy and chronic pain patients is warranted. KW - pain regulation KW - emotion regulation KW - acceptance KW - cognitive strategies KW - acute pain KW - acceptance-based strategy KW - psychological modulation of pain KW - pain ratings Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-207220 SN - 1664-1078 VL - 11 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Suchotzki, Kristina A1 - May, Heidi A1 - Gamer, Matthias T1 - No effect of moderate alcohol intake on the detection of concealed identity information in the laboratory JF - Scientific Reports N2 - The Concealed Information Test (CIT) enables the detection of certain (e.g., crime-relevant or personal) information, even if participants aim to conceal their knowledge. The current preregistered study investigated whether previously observed impairing effects of alcohol intoxication on participants' performance in a reaction time CIT (RT CIT) field study also translate to a laboratory environment. In contrast to the previous study of Suchotzki and Gamer (Sci Rep 8:7825, 2018) in which alcohol consumption was voluntary and self-administered, the current study used a randomized assignment of participants to either an alcohol group (n=88; receiving a drink with 3 cl alcohol) or a sober control group (n=89; receiving a drink with just some alcohol drops to hide group assignment). After drink administration, participants completed an RT CIT, in which they were instructed to hide knowledge of their own identity. Blood alcohol concentration (BAC) was estimated via breath alcohol ratio. In contrast to the previous field study, results revealed no differences in CIT-performance between intoxicated and sober participants. Aside from questioning the robustness of the result of the previous field study, our results also point to a number of interesting theoretical explanations for the discrepancy between both results, which are elaborated in the discussion. KW - Concealed Information Test KW - concealed identity information KW - CIT Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-231208 VL - 10 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Wirth, Robert A1 - Foerster, Anna A1 - Kunde, Wilfried A1 - Pfister, Roland T1 - Design choices: Empirical recommendations for designing two-dimensional finger-tracking experiments JF - Behavior Research Methods N2 - The continuous tracking of mouse or finger movements has become an increasingly popular research method for investigating cognitive and motivational processes such as decision-making, action-planning, and executive functions. In the present paper, we evaluate and discuss how apparently trivial design choices of researchers may impact participants’ behavior and, consequently, a study’s results. We first provide a thorough comparison of mouse- and finger-tracking setups on the basis of a Simon task. We then vary a comprehensive set of design factors, including spatial layout, movement extent, time of stimulus onset, size of the target areas, and hit detection in a finger-tracking variant of this task. We explore the impact of these variations on a broad spectrum of movement parameters that are typically used to describe movement trajectories. Based on our findings, we suggest several recommendations for best practice that avoid some of the pitfalls of the methodology. Keeping these recommendations in mind will allow for informed decisions when planning and conducting future tracking experiments. KW - movement tracking KW - experimental design KW - Simon task KW - measures Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-235569 VL - 52 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Cao, Liyu A1 - Steinborn, Michael A1 - Kunde, Wilfried A1 - Haendel, Barbara T1 - Action force modulates action binding: evidence for a multisensory information integration explanation JF - Experimental Brain Research N2 - Action binding refers to the observation that the perceived time of an action (e.g., a keypress) is shifted towards the distal sensory feedback (usually a sound) triggered by that action. Surprisingly, the role of somatosensory feedback for this phe-nomenon has been largely ignored. We fill this gap by showing that the somatosensory feedback, indexed by keypress peak force, is functional in judging keypress time. Specifically, the strength of somatosensory feedback is positively correlated with reported keypress time when the keypress is not associated with an auditory feedback and negatively correlated when the keypress triggers an auditory feedback. The result is consistent with the view that the reported keypress time is shaped by sensory information from different modalities. Moreover, individual differences in action binding can be explained by a sensory information weighting between somatosensory and auditory feedback. At the group level, increasing the strength of somatosensory feedback can decrease action binding to a level not being detected statistically. Therefore, a multisensory information integration account (between somatosensory and auditory inputs) explains action binding at both a group level and an individual level. KW - action binding KW - force KW - somatosensory feedback KW - multisensory processing Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-232534 SN - 0014-4819 VL - 238 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Düking, Peter A1 - Tafler, Marie A1 - Wallmann-Sperlich, Birgit A1 - Sperlich, Billy A1 - Kleih, Sonja T1 - Behavior Change Techniques in Wrist-Worn Wearables to Promote Physical Activity: Content Analysis JF - JMIR Mhealth and Uhealth N2 - Background: Decreasing levels of physical activity (PA) increase the incidences of noncommunicable diseases, obesity, and mortality. To counteract these developments, interventions aiming to increase PA are urgently needed. Mobile health (mHealth) solutions such as wearable sensors (wearables) may assist with an improvement in PA. Objective: The aim of this study is to examine which behavior change techniques (BCTs) are incorporated in currently available commercial high-end wearables that target users’ PA behavior. Methods: The BCTs incorporated in 5 different high-end wearables (Apple Watch Series 3, Garmin Vívoactive 3, Fitbit Versa, Xiaomi Amazfit Stratos 2, and Polar M600) were assessed by 2 researchers using the BCT Taxonomy version 1 (BCTTv1). Effectiveness of the incorporated BCTs in promoting PA behavior was assessed by a content analysis of the existing literature. Results: The most common BCTs were goal setting (behavior), action planning, review behavior goal(s), discrepancy between current behavior and goal, feedback on behavior, self-monitoring of behavior, and biofeedback. Fitbit Versa, Garmin Vívoactive 3, Apple Watch Series 3, Polar M600, and Xiaomi Amazfit Stratos 2 incorporated 17, 16, 12, 11, and 11 BCTs, respectively, which are proven to effectively promote PA. Conclusions: Wearables employ different numbers and combinations of BCTs, which might impact their effectiveness in improving PA. To promote PA by employing wearables, we encourage researchers to develop a taxonomy specifically designed to assess BCTs incorporated in wearables. We also encourage manufacturers to customize BCTs based on the targeted populations. KW - cardiorespiratory fitness KW - innovation KW - smartwatch KW - technology KW - wearable KW - eHealth KW - mHealth Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-230556 VL - 8 IS - 11 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Hauf, Juliane E. K. A1 - Nieding, Gerhild A1 - Seger, Benedikt T. T1 - The development of dynamic perceptual simulations during sentence comprehension JF - Cognitive Processing N2 - Based on an embodied account of language comprehension, this study investigated the dynamic characteristics of children and adults’ perceptual simulations during sentence comprehension, using a novel paradigm to assess the perceptual simulation of objects moving up and down a vertical axis. The participants comprised adults (N = 40) and 6-, 8-, and 10-year-old children (N = 116). After listening in experimental trials to sentences implying that objects moved upward or downward, the participants were shown pictures and had to decide as quickly as possible whether the objects depicted had been mentioned in the sentences. The target pictures moved either up or down and then stopped in the middle of the screen. All age groups’ reaction times were found to be shorter when the objects moved in the directions that the sentences implied. Age exerted no developmental effect on reaction times. The findings suggest that dynamic perceptual simulations are fundamental to language comprehension in text recipients aged 6 and older. KW - embodied cognition KW - sentence comprehension KW - dynamic perceptual simulation; KW - children KW - adults Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-283665 VL - 21 IS - 2 ER - TY - THES A1 - Menne, Isabelle M. T1 - Facing Social Robots – Emotional Reactions towards Social Robots N2 - Ein Army Colonel empfindet Mitleid mit einem Roboter, der versuchsweise Landminen entschärft und deklariert den Test als inhuman (Garreau, 2007). Roboter bekommen militärische Beförderungen, Beerdigungen und Ehrenmedaillen (Garreau, 2007; Carpenter, 2013). Ein Schildkrötenroboter wird entwickelt, um Kindern beizubringen, Roboter gut zu behandeln (Ackermann, 2018). Der humanoide Roboter Sophia wurde erst kürzlich Saudi-Arabischer Staatsbürger und es gibt bereits Debatten, ob Roboter Rechte bekommen sollen (Delcker, 2018). Diese und ähnliche Entwicklungen zeigen schon jetzt die Bedeutsamkeit von Robotern und die emotionale Wirkung die diese auslösen. Dennoch scheinen sich diese emotionalen Reaktionen auf einer anderen Ebene abzuspielen, gemessen an Kommentaren in Internetforen. Dort ist oftmals die Rede davon, wieso jemand überhaupt emotional auf einen Roboter reagieren kann. Tatsächlich ist es, rein rational gesehen, schwierig zu erklären, warum Menschen mit einer leblosen (‚mindless‘) Maschine mitfühlen sollten. Und dennoch zeugen nicht nur oben genannte Berichte, sondern auch erste wissenschaftliche Studien (z.B. Rosenthal- von der Pütten et al., 2013) von dem emotionalen Einfluss den Roboter auf Menschen haben können. Trotz der Bedeutsamkeit der Erforschung emotionaler Reaktionen auf Roboter existieren bislang wenige wissenschaftliche Studien hierzu. Tatsächlich identifizierten Kappas, Krumhuber und Küster (2013) die systematische Analyse und Evaluation sozialer Reaktionen auf Roboter als eine der größten Herausforderungen der affektiven Mensch-Roboter Interaktion. Nach Scherer (2001; 2005) bestehen Emotionen aus der Koordination und Synchronisation verschiedener Komponenten, die miteinander verknüpft sind. Motorischer Ausdruck (Mimik), subjektives Erleben, Handlungstendenzen, physiologische und kognitive Komponenten gehören hierzu. Um eine Emotion vollständig zu erfassen, müssten all diese Komponenten gemessen werden, jedoch wurde eine solch umfassende Analyse bisher noch nie durchgeführt (Scherer, 2005). Hauptsächlich werden Fragebögen eingesetzt (vgl. Bethel & Murphy, 2010), die allerdings meist nur das subjektive Erleben abfragen. Bakeman und Gottman (1997) geben sogar an, dass nur etwa 8% der psychologischen Forschung auf Verhaltensdaten basiert, obwohl die Psychologie traditionell als das ‚Studium von Psyche und Verhalten‘ (American Psychological Association, 2018) definiert wird. Die Messung anderer Emotionskomponenten ist selten. Zudem sind Fragebögen mit einer Reihe von Nachteilen behaftet (Austin, Deary, Gibson, McGregor, Dent, 1998; Fan et al., 2006; Wilcox, 2011). Bethel und Murphy (2010) als auch Arkin und Moshkina (2015) plädieren für einen Multi-Methodenansatz um ein umfassenderes Verständnis von affektiven Prozessen in der Mensch-Roboter Interaktion zu erlangen. Das Hauptziel der vorliegenden Dissertation ist es daher, mithilfe eines Multi-Methodenansatzes verschiedene Komponenten von Emotionen (motorischer Ausdruck, subjektive Gefühlskomponente, Handlungstendenzen) zu erfassen und so zu einem vollständigeren und tiefgreifenderem Bild emotionaler Prozesse auf Roboter beizutragen. Um dieses Ziel zu erreichen, wurden drei experimentelle Studien mit insgesamt 491 Teilnehmern durchgeführt. Mit unterschiedlichen Ebenen der „apparent reality“ (Frijda, 2007) sowie Macht / Kontrolle über die Situation (vgl. Scherer & Ellgring, 2007) wurde untersucht, inwiefern sich Intensität und Qualität emotionaler Reaktionen auf Roboter ändern und welche weiteren Faktoren (Aussehen des Roboters, emotionale Expressivität des Roboters, Behandlung des Roboters, Autoritätsstatus des Roboters) Einfluss ausüben. Experiment 1 basierte auf Videos, die verschiedene Arten von Robotern (tierähnlich, anthropomorph, maschinenartig), die entweder emotional expressiv waren oder nicht (an / aus) in verschiedenen Situationen (freundliche Behandlung des Roboters vs. Misshandlung) zeigten. Fragebögen über selbstberichtete Gefühle und die motorisch-expressive Komponente von Emotionen: Mimik (vgl. Scherer, 2005) wurden analysiert. Das Facial Action Coding System (Ekman, Friesen, & Hager, 2002), die umfassendste und am weitesten verbreitete Methode zur objektiven Untersuchung von Mimik, wurde hierfür verwendet. Die Ergebnisse zeigten, dass die Probanden Gesichtsausdrücke (Action Unit [AU] 12 und AUs, die mit positiven Emotionen assoziiert sind, sowie AU 4 und AUs, die mit negativen Emotionen assoziiert sind) sowie selbstberichtete Gefühle in Übereinstimmung mit der Valenz der in den Videos gezeigten Behandlung zeigten. Bei emotional expressiven Robotern konnten stärkere emotionale Reaktionen beobachtet werden als bei nicht-expressiven Robotern. Der tierähnliche Roboter Pleo erfuhr in der Misshandlungs-Bedingung am meisten Mitleid, Empathie, negative Gefühle und Traurigkeit, gefolgt vom anthropomorphen Roboter Reeti und am wenigsten für den maschinenartigen Roboter Roomba. Roomba wurde am meisten Antipathie zugeschrieben. Die Ergebnisse knüpfen an frühere Forschungen an (z.B. Krach et al., 2008; Menne & Schwab, 2018; Riek et al., 2009; Rosenthal-von der Pütten et al., 2013) und zeigen das Potenzial der Mimik für eine natürliche Mensch-Roboter Interaktion. Experiment 2 und Experiment 3 übertrugen die klassischen Experimente von Milgram (1963; 1974) zum Thema Gehorsam in den Kontext der Mensch-Roboter Interaktion. Die Gehorsamkeitsstudien von Milgram wurden als sehr geeignet erachtet, um das Ausmaß der Empathie gegenüber einem Roboter im Verhältnis zum Gehorsam gegenüber einem Roboter zu untersuchen. Experiment 2 unterschied sich von Experiment 3 in der Ebene der „apparent reality“ (Frijda, 2007): in Anlehnung an Milgram (1963) wurde eine rein text-basierte Studie (Experiment 2) einer live Mensch-Roboter Interaktion (Experiment 3) gegenübergestellt. Während die abhängigen Variablen von Experiment 2 aus den Selbstberichten emotionaler Gefühle sowie Einschätzungen des hypothetischen Verhaltens bestand, erfasste Experiment 3 subjektive Gefühle sowie reales Verhalten (Reaktionszeit: Dauer des Zögerns; Gehorsamkeitsrate; Anzahl der Proteste; Mimik) der Teilnehmer. Beide Experimente untersuchten den Einfluss der Faktoren „Autoritätsstatus“ (hoch / niedrig) des Roboters, der die Befehle erteilt (Nao) und die emotionale Expressivität (an / aus) des Roboters, der die Strafen erhält (Pleo). Die subjektiven Gefühle der Teilnehmer aus Experiment 2 unterschieden sich zwischen den Gruppen nicht. Darüber hinaus gaben nur wenige Teilnehmer (20.2%) an, dass sie den „Opfer“-Roboter definitiv bestrafen würden. Ein ähnliches Ergebnis fand auch Milgram (1963). Das reale Verhalten von Versuchsteilnehmern in Milgrams‘ Labor-Experiment unterschied sich jedoch von Einschätzungen hypothetischen Verhaltens von Teilnehmern, denen Milgram das Experiment nur beschrieben hatte. Ebenso lassen Kommentare von Teilnehmern aus Experiment 2 darauf schließen, dass das beschriebene Szenario möglicherweise als fiktiv eingestuft wurde und Einschätzungen von hypothetischem Verhalten daher kein realistisches Bild realen Verhaltens gegenüber Roboter in einer live Interaktion zeichnen können. Daher wurde ein weiteres Experiment (Experiment 3) mit einer Live Interaktion mit einem Roboter als Autoritätsfigur (hoher Autoritätsstatus vs. niedriger) und einem weiteren Roboter als „Opfer“ (emotional expressiv vs. nicht expressiv) durchgeführt. Es wurden Gruppenunterschiede in Fragebögen über emotionale Reaktionen gefunden. Dem emotional expressiven Roboter wurde mehr Empathie entgegengebracht und es wurde mehr Freude und weniger Antipathie berichtet als gegenüber einem nicht-expressiven Roboter. Außerdem konnten Gesichtsausdrücke beobachtet werden, die mit negativen Emotionen assoziiert sind während Probanden Nao’s Befehl ausführten und Pleo bestraften. Obwohl Probanden tendenziell länger zögerten, wenn sie einen emotional expressiven Roboter bestrafen sollten und der Befehl von einem Roboter mit niedrigem Autoritätsstatus kam, wurde dieser Unterschied nicht signifikant. Zudem waren alle bis auf einen Probanden gehorsam und bestraften Pleo, wie vom Nao Roboter befohlen. Dieses Ergebnis steht in starkem Gegensatz zu dem selbstberichteten hypothetischen Verhalten der Teilnehmer aus Experiment 2 und unterstützt die Annahme, dass die Einschätzungen von hypothetischem Verhalten in einem Mensch-Roboter-Gehorsamkeitsszenario nicht zuverlässig sind für echtes Verhalten in einer live Mensch-Roboter Interaktion. Situative Variablen, wie z.B. der Gehorsam gegenüber Autoritäten, sogar gegenüber einem Roboter, scheinen stärker zu sein als Empathie für einen Roboter. Dieser Befund knüpft an andere Studien an (z.B. Bartneck & Hu, 2008; Geiskkovitch et al., 2016; Menne, 2017; Slater et al., 2006), eröffnet neue Erkenntnisse zum Einfluss von Robotern, zeigt aber auch auf, dass die Wahl einer Methode um Empathie für einen Roboter zu evozieren eine nicht triviale Angelegenheit ist (vgl. Geiskkovitch et al., 2016; vgl. Milgram, 1965). Insgesamt stützen die Ergebnisse die Annahme, dass die emotionalen Reaktionen auf Roboter tiefgreifend sind und sich sowohl auf der subjektiven Ebene als auch in der motorischen Komponente zeigen. Menschen reagieren emotional auf einen Roboter, der emotional expressiv ist und eher weniger wie eine Maschine aussieht. Sie empfinden Empathie und negative Gefühle, wenn ein Roboter misshandelt wird und diese emotionalen Reaktionen spiegeln sich in der Mimik. Darüber hinaus unterscheiden sich die Einschätzungen von Menschen über ihr eigenes hypothetisches Verhalten von ihrem tatsächlichen Verhalten, weshalb videobasierte oder live Interaktionen zur Analyse realer Verhaltensreaktionen empfohlen wird. Die Ankunft sozialer Roboter in der Gesellschaft führt zu nie dagewesenen Fragen und diese Dissertation liefert einen ersten Schritt zum Verständnis dieser neuen Herausforderungen. N2 - An Army Colonel feels sorry for a robot that defuses landmines on a trial basis and declares the test inhumane (Garreau, 2007). Robots receive military promotions, funerals and medals of honor (Garreau, 2007; Carpenter, 2013). A turtle robot is being developed to teach children to treat robots well (Ackermann, 2018). The humanoid robot Sophia recently became a Saudi Arabian citizen and there are now debates whether robots should have rights (Delcker, 2018). These and similar developments already show the importance of robots and the emotional impact they have. Nevertheless, these emotional reactions seem to take place on a different level, judging by comments in internet forums alone: Most often, emotional reactions towards robots are questioned if not denied at all. In fact, from a purely rational point of view, it is difficult to explain why people should empathize with a mindless machine. However, not only the reports mentioned above but also first scientific studies (e.g. Rosenthal- von der Pütten et al., 2013) bear witness to the emotional influence of robots on humans. Despite the importance of researching emotional reactions towards robots, there are few scientific studies on this subject. In fact, Kappas, Krumhuber and Küster (2013) identified effective testing and evaluation of social reactions towards robots as one of the major challenges of affective Human-Robot Interaction (HRI). According to Scherer (2001; 2005), emotions consist of the coordination and synchronization of different components that are linked to each other. These include motor expression (facial expressions), subjective experience, action tendencies, physiological and cognitive components. To fully capture an emotion, all these components would have to be measured, but such a comprehensive analysis has never been performed (Scherer, 2005). Primarily, questionnaires are used (cf. Bethel & Murphy, 2010) but most of them only capture subjective experiences. Bakeman and Gottman (1997) even state that only about 8% of psychological research is based on behavioral data, although psychology is traditionally defined as the 'study of the mind and behavior' (American Psychological Association, 2018). The measurement of other emotional components is rare. In addition, questionnaires have a number of disadvantages (Austin, Deary, Gibson, McGregor, Dent, 1998; Fan et al., 2006; Wilcox, 2011). Bethel and Murphy (2010) as well as Arkin and Moshkina (2015) argue for a multi-method approach to achieve a more comprehensive understanding of affective processes in HRI. The main goal of this dissertation is therefore to use a multi-method approach to capture different components of emotions (motor expression, subjective feeling component, action tendencies) and thus contribute to a more complete and profound picture of emotional processes towards robots. To achieve this goal, three experimental studies were conducted with a total of 491 participants. With different levels of ‘apparent reality’ (Frijda, 2007) and power/control over the situation (cf. Scherer & Ellgring, 2007), the extent to which the intensity and quality of emotional responses to robots change were investigated as well as the influence of other factors (appearance of the robot, emotional expressivity of the robot, treatment of the robot, authority status of the robot). Experiment 1 was based on videos showing different types of robots (animal-like, anthropomorphic, machine-like) in different situations (friendly treatment of the robot vs. torture treatment) while being either emotionally expressive or not. Self-reports of feelings as well as the motoric-expressive component of emotion: facial expressions (cf. Scherer, 2005) were analyzed. The Facial Action Coding System (Ekman, Friesen, & Hager, 2002), the most comprehensive and most widely used method for objectively assessing facial expressions, was utilized for this purpose. Results showed that participants displayed facial expressions (Action Unit [AU] 12 and AUs associated with positive emotions as well as AU 4 and AUs associated with negative emotions) as well as self-reported feelings in line with the valence of the treatment shown in the videos. Stronger emotional reactions could be observed for emotionally expressive robots than non-expressive robots. Most pity, empathy, negative feelings and sadness were reported for the animal-like robot Pleo while watching it being tortured, followed by the anthropomorphic robot Reeti and least for the machine-like robot Roomba. Most antipathy was attributed to Roomba. The findings are in line with previous research (e.g., Krach et al., 2008; Menne & Schwab, 2018; Riek et al., 2009; Rosenthal-von der Pütten et al., 2013) and show facial expressions’ potential for a natural HRI. Experiment 2 and Experiment 3 transferred Milgram’s classic experiments (1963; 1974) on obedience into the context of HRI. Milgram’s obedience studies were deemed highly suitable to study the extent of empathy towards a robot in relation to obedience to a robot. Experiment 2 differed from Experiment 3 in the level of ‘apparent reality’ (Frijda, 2007): based on Milgram (1963), a purely text-based study (Experiment 2) was compared with a live HRI (Experiment 3). While the dependent variables of Experiment 2 consisted of self-reports of emotional feelings and assessments of hypothetical behavior, Experiment 3 measured subjective feelings and real behavior (reaction time: duration of hesitation; obedience rate; number of protests; facial expressions) of the participants. Both experiments examined the influence of the factors "authority status" (high / low) of the robot giving the orders (Nao) and the emotional expressivity (on / off) of the robot receiving the punishments (Pleo). The subjective feelings of the participants from Experiment 2 did not differ between the groups. In addition, only few participants (20.2%) stated that they would definitely punish the "victim" robot. Milgram (1963) found a similar result. However, the real behavior of participants in Milgram's laboratory experiment differed from the estimates of hypothetical behavior of participants to whom Milgram had only described the experiment. Similarly, comments from participants in Experiment 2 suggest that the scenario described may have been considered fictitious and that assessments of hypothetical behavior may not provide a realistic picture of real behavior towards robots in a live interaction. Therefore, another experiment (Experiment 3) was performed with a live interaction with a robot as authority figure (high authority status vs. low) and another robot as "victim" (emotional expressive vs. non-expressive). Group differences were found in questionnaires on emotional responses. More empathy was shown for the emotionally expressive robot and more joy and less antipathy was reported than for a non-expressive robot. In addition, facial expressions associated with negative emotions could be observed while subjects executed Nao's command and punished Pleo. Although subjects tended to hesitate longer when punishing an emotionally expressive robot and the order came from a robot with low authority status, this difference did not reach significance. Furthermore, all but one subject were obedient and punished Pleo as commanded by the Nao robot. This result stands in stark contrast to the self-reported hypothetical behavior of the participants from Experiment 2 and supports the assumption that the assessments of hypothetical behavior in a Human-Robot obedience scenario are not reliable for real behavior in a live HRI. Situational variables, such as obedience to authorities, even to a robot, seem to be stronger than empathy for a robot. This finding is in line with previous studies (e.g. Bartneck & Hu, 2008; Geiskkovitch et al., 2016; Menne, 2017; Slater et al., 2006), opens up new insights into the influence of robots, but also shows that the choice of a method to evoke empathy for a robot is not a trivial matter (cf. Geiskkovitch et al., 2016; cf. Milgram, 1965). Overall, the results support the assumption that emotional reactions to robots are profound and manifest both at the subjective level and in the motor component. Humans react emotionally to a robot that is emotionally expressive and looks less like a machine. They feel empathy and negative feelings when a robot is abused and these emotional reactions are reflected in facial expressions. In addition, people's assessments of their own hypothetical behavior differ from their actual behavior, which is why video-based or live interactions are recommended for analyzing real behavioral responses. The arrival of social robots in society leads to unprecedented questions and this dissertation provides a first step towards understanding these new challenges. N2 - Are there emotional reactions towards social robots? Could you love a robot? Or, put the other way round: Could you mistreat a robot, tear it apart and sell it? Media reports people honoring military robots with funerals, mourning the “death” of a robotic dog, and granting the humanoid robot Sophia citizenship. But how profound are these reactions? Three experiments take a closer look on emotional reactions towards social robots by investigating the subjective experience of people as well as the motor expressive level. Contexts of varying degrees of Human-Robot Interaction (HRI) sketch a nuanced picture of emotions towards social robots that encompass conscious as well as unconscious reactions. The findings advance the understanding of affective experiences in HRI. It also turns the initial question into: Can emotional reactions towards social robots even be avoided? T2 - Im Angesicht sozialer Roboter - Emotionale Reaktionen angesichts sozialer Roboter KW - Roboter KW - social robot KW - emotion KW - FACS KW - Facial Action Coding System KW - facial expressions KW - emotional reaction KW - Human-Robot Interaction KW - HRI KW - obedience KW - empathy KW - Gefühl KW - Mimik KW - Mensch-Maschine-Kommunikation Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-187131 SN - 978-3-95826-120-4 SN - 978-3-95826-121-1 N1 - Parallel erschienen als Druckausgabe in Würzburg University Press, 978-3-95826-120-4, 27,80 EUR. PB - Würzburg University Press CY - Würzburg ET - 1. Auflage ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Mocke, Viola A1 - Weller, Lisa A1 - Frings, Christian A1 - Rothermund, Klaus A1 - Kunde, Wilfried T1 - Task relevance determines binding of effect features in action planning JF - Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics N2 - Action planning can be construed as the temporary binding of features of perceptual action effects. While previous research demonstrated binding for task-relevant, body-related effect features, the role of task-irrelevant or environment-related effect features in action planning is less clear. Here, we studied whether task-relevance or body-relatedness determines feature binding in action planning. Participants planned an action A, but before executing it initiated an intermediate action B. Each action relied on a body-related effect feature (index vs. middle finger movement) and an environment-related effect feature (cursor movement towards vs. away from a reference object). In Experiments 1 and 2, both effects were task-relevant. Performance in action B suffered from partial feature overlap with action A compared to full feature repetition or alternation, which is in line with binding of both features while planning action A. Importantly, this cost disappeared when all features were available but only body-related features were task-relevant (Experiment 3). When only the environment-related effect of action A was known in advance, action B benefitted when it aimed at the same (vs. a different) environment-related effect (Experiment 4). Consequently, the present results support the idea that task relevance determines whether binding of body-related and environment-related effect features takes place while the pre-activation of environment-related features without binding them primes feature-overlapping actions. KW - action planning KW - motor control KW - binding KW - effect anticipations Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-231906 SN - 1943-3921 VL - 82 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Liesner, Marvin A1 - Kunde, Wilfried T1 - Suppression of mutually incompatible proprioceptive and visual action effects in tool use JF - PLoS One N2 - Movements of a tool typically diverge from the movements of the hand manipulating that tool, such as when operating a pivotal lever where tool and hand move in opposite directions. Previous studies suggest that humans are often unaware of the position or movements of their effective body part (mostly the hand) in such situations. It has been suggested that this might be due to a "haptic neglect" of bodily sensations to decrease the interference of representations of body and tool movements. However, in principle this interference could also be decreased by neglecting sensations regarding the tool and focusing instead on body movements. While in most tool use situations the tool-related action effects are task-relevant and thus suppression of body-related rather than tool-related sensations is more beneficial for successful goal achievement, we manipulated this task-relevance in a controlled experiment. The results showed that visual, tool-related effect representations can be suppressed just as proprioceptive, body-related ones in situations where effect representations interfere, given that task-relevance of body-related effects is increased relative to tool-related ones. KW - movement KW - tool use KW - effects Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-231250 VL - 15 IS - 11 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Rodrigues, Johannes A1 - Liesner, Marvin A1 - Reutter, Mario A1 - Mussel, Patrick A1 - Hewig, Johannes T1 - It's costly punishment, not altruistic: Low midfrontal theta and state anger predict punishment JF - Psychophysiology N2 - Punishment in economic games has been interpreted as “altruistic.” However, it was shown that punishment is related to trait anger instead of trait altruism in a third‐party dictator game if compensation is also available. Here, we investigated the influence of state anger on punishment and compensation in the third‐party dictator game. Therefore, we used movie sequences for emotional priming, including the target states anger, happy, and neutral. We measured the Feedback‐Related Negativity (FRN) and midfrontal theta band activation, to investigate an electro‐cortical correlate of the processing of fair and unfair offers. Also, we assessed single‐trial FRN and midfrontal theta band activation as a predictor for punishment and compensation. We found that punishment was linked to state anger. Midfrontal theta band activation, which has previously been linked to altruistic acts and cognitive control, predicted less punishment. Additionally, trait anger led to enhanced FRN for unfair offers. This led to the interpretation that the FRN depicts the evaluation of fairness, while midfrontal theta band activation captures an aspect of cognitive control and altruistic motivation. We conclude that we need to redefine “altruistic punishment” into “costly punishment,” as no direct link of altruism and punishment is given. Additionally, midfrontal theta band activation complements the FRN and offers additional insights into complex responses and decision processes, especially as a single trial predictor.  KW - altruistic compensation KW - costly punishment KW - FRN KW - midfrontal theta activation KW - third party dictator game Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-214696 VL - 57 IS - 8 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Weiß, Martin A1 - Rodrigues, Johannes A1 - Paelecke, Marko A1 - Hewig, Johannes T1 - We, Them, and It: Dictator Game Offers Depend on Hierarchical Social Status, Artificial Intelligence, and Social Dominance JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - We investigated the influence of social status on behavior in a modified dictator game (DG). Since the DG contains an inherent dominance gradient, we examined the relationship between dictator decisions and recipient status, which was operationalized by three social identities and an artificial intelligence (AI). Additionally, we examined the predictive value of social dominance orientation (SDO) on the behavior of dictators toward the different social and non-social hierarchical recipients. A multilevel model analysis showed that recipients with the same status as the dictator benefited the most and the artificial intelligence the least. Furthermore, SDO, regardless of social status, predicted behavior toward recipients in such a way that higher dominance was associated with lower dictator offers. In summary, participants treated other persons of higher and lower status equally, those of equal status better and, above all, an algorithm worst. The large proportion of female participants and the limited variance of SDO should be taken into account with regard to the results of individual differences in SDO. KW - decision-making KW - dictator game KW - personality KW - social dominance KW - social status Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-218168 SN - 1664-1078 VL - 11 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Leopold, Karolina A1 - Bauer, Michael A1 - Bechdolf, Andreas A1 - Correll, Christoph U. A1 - Holtmann, Martin A1 - Juckel, Georg A1 - Lambert, Martin A1 - Meyer, Thomas D. A1 - Pfeiffer, Steffi A1 - Kittel‐Schneider, Sarah A1 - Reif, Andreas A1 - Stamm, Thomas J. A1 - Rottmann‐Wolf, Maren A1 - Mathiebe, Josephine A1 - Kellmann, Eva L. A1 - Ritter, Philipp A1 - Krüger‐Özgürdal, Seza A1 - Karow, Anne A1 - Sondergeld, Lene‐Marie A1 - Roessner, Veit A1 - Sauer, Cathrin A1 - Pfennig, Andrea T1 - Efficacy of cognitive‐behavioral group therapy in patients at risk for serious mental illness presenting with subthreshold bipolar symptoms: Results from a prespecified interim analysis of a multicenter, randomized, controlled study JF - Bipolar Disorders N2 - Objective Most patients with bipolar disorders (BD) exhibit prodromal symptoms before a first (hypo)manic episode. Patients with clinically significant symptoms fulfilling at‐risk criteria for serious mental illness (SMI) require effective and safe treatment. Cognitive‐behavioral psychotherapy (CBT) has shown promising results in early stages of BD and in patients at high risk for psychosis. We aimed to investigate whether group CBT can improve symptoms and functional deficits in young patients at risk for SMI presenting with subthreshold bipolar symptoms. Method In a multicenter, randomized, controlled trial, patients at clinical risk for SMI presenting with subthreshold bipolar symptoms aged 15‐30 years were randomized to 14 weeks of at‐risk for BD‐specific group CBT or unstructured group meetings. Primary efficacy endpoints were differences in affective symptomatology and psychosocial functioning at 14 weeks. At‐risk status was defined as a combination of subthreshold bipolar symptomatology, reduction of psychosocial functioning and a family history for (schizo)affective disorders. A prespecified interim analysis was conducted at 75% of the targeted sample. Results Of 128 screened participants, 75 were randomized to group CBT (n = 38, completers = 65.8%) vs unstructured group meetings (n = 37, completers = 78.4%). Affective symptomatology and psychosocial functioning improved significantly at week 14 (P < .001) and during 6 months (P < .001) in both groups, without significant between‐group differences. Findings are limited by the interim character of the analysis, the use of not fully validated early detection interviews, a newly adapted intervention manual, and the substantial drop‐outs. Conclusions Results suggest that young patients at‐risk for SMI presenting with subthreshold bipolar symptoms benefit from early group sessions. The degree of specificity and psychotherapeutic interaction needed requires clarification. KW - at‐risk KW - bipolar disorder KW - CBT KW - early intervention KW - group treatment KW - prodromal KW - serious mental illness KW - subthreshold bipolar Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-215469 VL - 22 IS - 5 SP - 517 EP - 529 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Breil, Christina A1 - Böckler, Anne T1 - The Lens Shapes the View: on Task Dependency in ToM Research JF - Current Behavioral Neuroscience Reports N2 - Purpose of Review This article provides an overview of current findings on Theory of Mind (ToM) in human children and adults and highlights the relationship between task specifications and their outcome in socio-cognitive research. Recent Findings ToM, the capacity to reason about and infer others’ mental states, develops progressively throughout childhood—the exact time course is still a matter of debate. Neuroimaging studies indicate the involvement of a widespread neuronal network during mentalizing, suggesting that ToM is a multifaceted process. Accordingly, the tasks and trainings that currently exist to investigate and enhance ToM are heterogeneous, and the outcomes largely depend on the paradigm that was used. Summary We argue for the implementation of multiple-task batteries in the assessment of socio-cognitive abilities. Decisions for a particular paradigm need to be carefully considered and justified. We want to emphasize the importance of targeted research on the relationship between task specifications and outcomes. KW - theory of mind KW - mentalizing KW - perspective taking KW - social cognition KW - social interaction KW - task dependency Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-232646 VL - 7 ER - TY - THES A1 - Förster, Anna T1 - Searching for truth in dishonesty: The cognitive architecture of lying T1 - Die Wahrheit in der Lüge: Der kognitive Aufbau des Lügens N2 - Honest actions predominate human behavior. From time to time, this general preference must yield to dishonest actions, which require an effortful process of overcoming initial honest response activation. This thesis presents three experimental series to elucidate this tug-of-war between honest and dishonest response tendencies in overtly committed instances of lies, thereby joining recent efforts to move from a sheer phenomenological perspective on dishonest responding as being more difficult than honest responding to a precise description of the underlying cognitive processes. The consideration of cognitive theories, empirical evidence, and paradigms from different research fields – dishonesty, cognitive control and sensorimotor stage models of information processing – lay the groundwork for the research questions and methodological approach of this thesis. The experiments pinpoint the underlying conflict of dishonest responding in the central, capacity-limited stage of information processing (Experiments 1 to 4), but they also demonstrate that cognitive control processes (Experiments 5 to 7) and the internalization of false alibis (Experiments 8 to 11) can reduce or even completely eliminate this conflict. The data reveals great flexibility at the cognitive basis of dishonest responding: On the one hand, dishonest responding appears to rely heavily on capacity-limited processes of response selection to overcome honest response tendencies alongside up- and downstream consequences of response activation and monitoring. On the other hand, agents have powerful tools to mitigate these effortful processes through control adaptation and false alibis. These results support and expand current theorizing of the cognitive underpinnings of dishonest responding. Furthermore, they are alerting from an applied perspective on the detection of lies, especially when considering the flexibility of even basic cognitive processes in the face of false alibis. A promising way to move forward from here would be a fine-grained discrimination of response activation, passive decay and active inhibition of honest representations in dishonest responding and the assessment of the adaptiveness of these processes. N2 - Menschen handeln und interagieren in der Regel entsprechend dem was sie als wahr erachten. Allerdings muss diese Präferenz hin und wieder unehrlichen Handlungen weichen. Die dafür notwendige Überwindung initial ehrlicher Antworttendenzen erweist sich als kognitiv aufwendig. Diese Thesis ergründet in drei Experimentalserien die Eigenschaften dieses Wettstreits ehrlicher und unehrlicher Antworttendenzen für offen ausgeführte Lügen. Damit reihen sie sich in jüngste Bestrebungen ein, Lügen nicht nur oberflächlich als schwierigere der beiden Handlungen zu beschreiben, sondern zu einer präzisen Charakterisierung der beteiligten kognitiven Prozesse zu gelangen. Die Forschungsfragen und das methodische Vorgehen dieser Thesis basieren dafür auf der gemeinsamen Betrachtung kognitiver Theorien, empirischer Evidenz und Paradigmen aus der Forschung zum Lügen, zur kognitiven Kontrolle und zu sensomotorischen Stadienmodellen der Informationsverarbeitung. Die Experimente lokalisieren den dem Lügen inhärenten Handlungskonflikt in der zentralen, kapazitätslimitierten Phase der Informationsverarbeitung (Experimente 1 bis 4), zeigen jedoch auch, dass dieser Konflikt sowohl durch kognitive Kontrollmechanismen (Experimente 5 bis 7) als auch durch das Verinnerlichen falscher Alibis (Experimente 8 bis 11) reduziert bzw. vollständig eliminiert werden kann. Die Daten offenbaren eine starke Flexibilität in der kognitiven Verarbeitung unehrlicher Handlungen: Einerseits scheint die Ausführung einer Lüge und die Überwindung wahrheitsgemäßer Handlungstendenzen besonders auf kapazitätslimitierte Selektionsprozesse zurückzugreifen, begleitet von vor- und nachgelagerten Aktivierungs- und Überwachungsprozessen. Andererseits können kognitive Kontrollmechanismen und falsche Alibis diese aufwändigen Prozesse entscheidend eindämmen. Diese Ergebnisse untermauern und erweitern bestehende Theorien zu den kognitiven Grundlagen des Lügens. Für angewandte Vorhaben im Bereich der Lügendetektion ist die beobachtete Flexibilität der kognitiven Verarbeitung angesichts falscher Alibis alarmierend. Ein vielversprechender Ansatz zur Weiterentwicklung in diesem Bereich wäre eine genaue Unterscheidung von Prozessen der Aktivierung, des passiven Zerfalls und der aktiven Inhibition wahrheitsgemäßer Repräsentationen beim Lügen und eine Bewertung der Anpassungsfähigkeit dieser Prozesse. KW - Lüge KW - Handlungsregulation KW - Kognition KW - Dishonesty KW - Lying KW - Lie detection KW - Cognitive control KW - Cognitive conflict KW - Capacity limitations KW - PRP KW - Täuschung KW - Lügendetektion Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-209730 ER - TY - THES A1 - Weiß, Martin T1 - The neural principles of behavior modification using socioemotional facial feedback cues in economic decision-making T1 - Die neuronalen Mechanismen der Verhaltensmodifikation durch sozio-emotionale faziale Feedbackreize bei ökonomischen Entscheidungen N2 - The present dissertation aims to shed light on different mechanisms of socio-emotional feedback in social decision-making situations. The objective is to evaluate emotional facial expressions as feedback stimuli, i.e., responses of interaction partners to certain social decisions. In addition to human faces, artificial emojis are also examined due to their relevance for modern digital communication. Previous research on the influence of emotional feedback suggests that a person's behavior can be effectively reinforced by rewarding stimuli. In the context of this dissertation, the differences in the feedback processing of human photographs and emojis, but also the evaluation of socially expected versus socially unexpected feedback were examined in detail in four studies. In addition to behavioral data, we used the electroencephalogram (EEG) in all studies to investigate neural correlates of social decision-making and emotional feedback. As the central paradigm, all studies were based on a modified ultimatum game. The game is structured as follows: there is a so-called proposer who holds a specific amount of money (e.g., 10 cents) and offers the responder a certain amount (e.g., 3 cents). The responder then decides whether to accept or reject the offer. In the version of the ultimatum game presented here, different types of proposers are introduced. After the participants have accepted or rejected in the role of the responder, the different proposers react to the participant’s decision with specific emotional facial expressions. Different feedback patterns are used for the individual experiments conducted in the course of this dissertation. In the first study, we investigated the influence of emotional feedback on decision-making in the modified version of the ultimatum game. We were able to show that a proposer who responds to the acceptance of an offer with a smiling face achieves more accepted offers overall than a control proposer who responds to both accepted and rejected offers with a neutral facial expression. Consequently, the smile served as a positive reinforcement. Similarly, a sad expression in response to a rejected offer also resulted in higher acceptance rates as compared to the control identity, which could be considered an expression of compassion for that proposer. On a neuronal level, we could show that there are differences between simply looking at negative emotional stimuli (i.e., sad and angry faces) and their appearance as feedback stimuli after rejected offers in the modified ultimatum game. The so-called feedback-related negativity was reduced (i.e., more positive) when negative emotions appeared as feedback from the proposers. We argued that these findings might show that the participants wanted to punish the proposers by rejecting an offer for its unfairness and therefore the negative feedback met their expectations. The altered processing of negative emotional facial expressions in the ultimatum game could therefore indicate that the punishment is interpreted as successful. This includes the expectation that the interaction partner will change his behavior in the future and eventually make fairer offers. In the second study we wanted to show that smiling and sad emojis as feedback stimuli in the modified ultimatum game can also lead to increased acceptance rates. Contrary to our assumptions, this effect could not be observed. At the neural level as well, the findings did not correspond to our assumptions and differed strongly from those of the first study. One finding, however, was that the neural P3 component showed how the use of emojis as feedback stimuli particularly characterizes certain types of proposers. This is supported by the fact that the P3 is increased for the proposer who rewards an acceptance with a smile as well as for the proposer who reacts to rejection with a sad emoji compared to the neutral control proposer. The third study examined the discrepancy between the findings of the first and second study. Accordingly, both humans and emojis representing the different proposers were presented in the ultimatum game. In addition, emojis were selected that showed a higher similarity to known emojis from common messenger services compared to the second study. We were able to replicate that the proposers in the ultimatum game, who reward an acceptance of the offer with a smile, led to an increased acceptance rate compared to the neutral control proposers. This difference is independent of whether the proposers are represented by emojis or human faces. With regard to the neural correlates, we were able to demonstrate that emojis and human faces differ strongly in their neural processing. Emojis showed stronger activation than human faces in the face-processing N170 component, the feedback-related negativity and the P3 component. We concluded that the results of the N170 and feedback-related negativity could indicate a signal for missing social information of emojis compared to faces. The increased P3 amplitude for emojis might imply that emojis appear unexpectedly as reward stimuli in a social decision task compared to human faces. The last study of this project dealt with socially unexpected feedback. In comparison to the first three studies, new proposer identities were implemented. In particular, the focus was on a proposer who reacted to the rejection of an offer unexpectedly with a smile and to the acceptance with a neutral facial expression. According to the results, participants approach this unexpected smile through increased rejection, although it is accompanied by financial loss. In addition, as reported in studies one and three, we were able to show that proposers who respond to the acceptance of an offer with a smiling face and thus meet the expectations of the participants have higher offer acceptance rates than the control proposer. At the neuronal level, especially the feedback from the socially unexpected proposer led to an increased P3 amplitude, which indicates that smiling after rejection is attributed a special subjective importance. The experiments provide new insights into the social influence through emotional feedback and the processing of relevant social cues. Due to the conceptual similarity of the studies, it was possible to differentiate between stable findings and potentially stimulus-dependent deviations, thus creating a well-founded contribution to the current research. Therefore, the novel paradigm presented here, and the knowledge gained from it could also play an important role in the future for clinical questions dealing with limited social competencies. N2 - Die vorliegende Dissertation soll verschiedene Mechanismen des sozio-emotionalen Feedbacks in sozialen Entscheidungssituationen beleuchten. Ziel ist es, emotionale Gesichtsausdrücke als Feedbackreize, d.h. Reaktion des Gegenübers auf bestimmte soziale Entscheidungen, zu evaluieren. Neben menschlichen Gesichtern werden auch künstliche Emojis aufgrund ihrer Relevanz für die moderne digitale Kommunikation untersucht. Bisherige Forschungen zum Einfluss von emotionalem Feedback legen nahe, dass das Verhalten einer Person durch belohnende Hinweisreize erfolgreich verstärkt werden kann. Im Rahmen dieser Dissertation wurden daher vier Studien durchgeführt, die die Unterschiede in der Feedback-Verarbeitung von menschlichen Fotos und Emojis, aber auch die Bewertung von sozial erwartetem gegenüber sozial unerwartetem Feedback eingehend untersuchen. Zusätzlich zu den Verhaltensdaten verwendeten wir in allen Studien das Elektroenzephalogramm (EEG), um neuronale Korrelate sozialer Entscheidungen und emotionalen Feedbacks zu untersuchen. Als zentrales Paradigma wurde allen Studien ein modifiziertes Ultimatumspiel zugrunde gelegt. Dieses ist so aufgebaut, dass es einen sogenannten Anbieter gibt, der über einen bestimmten Geldbetrag verfügt (z.B. 10 Cent) und dem Empfänger einen gewissen Anteil davon anbietet (z.B. 3 Cent). Der Empfänger entscheidet daraufhin, ob er das Angebot annehmen oder ablehnen möchte. In der hier verwendeten Version des Ultimatumspiels werden dabei verschiedene Typen von Anbietern eingeführt. Nachdem die Versuchspersonen in der Rolle des Empfängers angenommen oder abgelehnt haben, reagieren die verschiedenen Anbieter mit spezifischen emotionalen Gesichtsausdrücken auf die Entscheidung der Versuchsperson. Für die einzelnen Experimente, die im Rahmen dieser Dissertation durchgeführt wurden, werden unterschiedliche Feedbackmuster angewandt. In der ersten Studie untersuchten wir den Einfluss des emotionalen Feedbacks auf die Entscheidungsfindung in der modifizierten Version des Ultimatumspiels. Wir konnten zeigen, dass im Ultimatumspiel ein Anbieter, der auf die Annahme eines Angebots mit einem lächelnden Gesicht reagiert, insgesamt mehr akzeptierte Angebote erzielt als der Anbieter der Kontrollbedingung, der sowohl auf angenommene als auch auf abgelehnte Angebote mit einem neutralen Gesichtsausdruck reagiert. Folglich wirkte das Lächeln als positive Verstärkung. In ähnlicher Weise führte ein trauriger Gesichtsausdruck als Reaktion auf ein abgelehntes Angebot ebenfalls zu höheren Annahmeraten als die Kontrollperson, was als Ausdruck von Mitgefühl für diesen Anbieter betrachtet werden könnte. Auf neuronaler Ebene konnten wir zeigen, dass es Unterschiede zwischen dem bloßen Betrachten negativer emotionaler Stimuli (d.h. trauriger und wütender Gesichter) und ihrem Auftreten als Feedback-Stimuli nach abgelehnten Angeboten im modifizierten Ultimatumspiel gibt. Die so genannte feedback-related negativity wurde reduziert (d.h. positiver), wenn negative Emotionen als Feedback von den Anbietern auftraten. Wir zogen aus den Ergebnissen den Schluss, dass die Versuchsteilnehmer die Anbieter bestrafen wollten, indem sie ein Angebot wegen seiner Unfairness ablehnten, und dass daher das negative Feedback ihren Erwartungen entsprach. Die veränderte Verarbeitung negativer emotionaler Gesichtsausdrücke im Ultimatumspiel könnte daher darauf hinweisen, dass die Bestrafung als erfolgreich interpretiert wird. Dies schließt die Erwartung ein, dass der Interaktionspartner sein Verhalten in Zukunft ändert und schließlich fairere Angebote machen sollte. In der zweiten Studie war es das Ziel zu zeigen, dass auch lächelnde und traurige Emojis als Feedback-Reize im modifizierten Ultimatumspiel zu erhöhten Annahmeraten führen können. Entgegen unseren Hypothesen konnte dieser Effekt jedoch nicht beobachtet werden. Auch auf der neuronalen Ebene entsprachen die Ergebnisse nicht unseren Annahmen und unterschieden sich stark von denen der ersten Studie. Eine Erkenntnis war jedoch, dass anhand der neuronalen P3-Komponente ersichtlich wurde, dass die Verwendung von Emojis als Feedback-Reize gewisse Typen von Anbietern besonders kennzeichnet. Dies wurde dadurch gezeigt, dass die P3 sowohl für den Anbieter, der eine Annahme mit einem Lächeln belohnt, als auch für den Anbieter, der auf eine Ablehnung mit einem traurigen Emoji reagiert, im Vergleich zum neutralen Kontrollanbieter erhöht ist. Die dritte Studie untersuchte die Diskrepanz zwischen den Ergebnissen der ersten und der zweiten Studie. Dementsprechend wurden sowohl Menschen als auch Emojis, die die Identitäten der Anbieter repräsentieren, im Ultimatumspiel präsentiert. Darüber hinaus wurden Emojis ausgewählt, die eine höhere Ähnlichkeit mit bekannten Emojis aus den üblichen Messenger-Diensten zeigten als in der zweiten Studie. Wir konnten replizieren, dass die Anbieter im Ultimatumspiel, die eine Annahme des Angebots mit einem Lächeln belohnen, zu einer höheren Annahmerate im Vergleich zu den neutralen Kontrollanbietern führen. Dieser Unterschied zeigte sich unabhängig davon, ob die Anbieter durch Emojis oder menschliche Gesichter repräsentiert wurden. In Bezug auf die neuronalen Korrelate konnten wir zeigen, dass sich Emojis und menschliche Gesichter in ihrer neuronalen Verarbeitung stark unterscheiden. Emojis zeigten sowohl in der gesichtsverarbeitenden N170-Komponente als auch in der feedback-related negativity eine stärkere Aktivierung als menschliche Gesichter. Wir schlussfolgerten daraus, dass die Ergebnisse der N170 und feedback-related negativity ein Signal für fehlende soziale Informationen von Emojis im Vergleich zu Gesichtern sein könnten. Die erhöhte P3-Amplitude für Emojis könnte dabei implizieren, dass Emojis im Vergleich zu menschlichen Gesichtern bei einer sozialen Entscheidungsaufgabe unerwartet als Belohnungsreiz erscheinen. Die letzte Studie dieses Projekts beschäftigte sich mit sozial unerwartetem Feedback. Im Vergleich zu den ersten drei Studien wurden neue Anbieteridentitäten implementiert. Im Mittelpunkt stand insbesondere ein Anbieter, der auf die Ablehnung eines Angebots unerwartet mit einem Lächeln und auf die Annahme mit einem neutralen Gesichtsausdruck reagierte. Den Ergebnissen zufolge nähern sich die Teilnehmer diesem unerwarteten Lächeln durch verstärkte Ablehnung an, obwohl es mit einem finanziellen Verlust einhergeht. Darüber hinaus konnten wir, wie in den Studien eins und drei berichtet, zeigen, dass Anbieter, die auf die Annahme eines Angebots mit einem lächelnden Gesicht reagieren und damit die Erwartungen der Teilnehmer erfüllen, höhere Angebotsannahmeraten haben als der Kontrollanbieter. Auf neuronaler Ebene führte insbesondere das Feedback des sozial unerwarteten Anbieters zu einer erhöhten P3-Amplitude, was darauf hinweist, dass dem Lächeln nach der Ablehnung eine besondere subjektive Bedeutung beigemessen wird. In ihrer Gesamtheit liefern die Experimente neue Erkenntnisse über den sozialen Einfluss durch emotionales Feedback und die Verarbeitung relevanter sozialer Signale. Aufgrund der konzeptionellen Ähnlichkeit der Studien ist es möglich, zwischen stabilen Befunden und möglicherweise reizabhängigen Abweichungen zu differenzieren und damit einen fundierten Beitrag zur aktuellen Forschung zu leisten. Das hier vorgestellte neuartige Paradigma und die daraus gewonnenen Erkenntnisse könnten daher in Zukunft auch für klinische Fragestellungen, die sich mit eingeschränkten sozialen Kompetenzen befassen, eine nicht unerhebliche Rolle spielen. KW - Entscheidungsverhalten KW - Affekt KW - decision-making KW - emotional feedback KW - emojis KW - ultimatum game KW - EEG KW - Electroencephalographie Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-208654 ER - TY - THES A1 - Hoffmann, Mareike T1 - Effector System Prioritization in Multitasking T1 - Effektorsystempriorisierung im Multitasking N2 - Multitasking, defined as performing more than one task at a time, typically yields performance decrements, for instance, in processing speed and accuracy. These performance costs are often distributed asymmetrically among the involved tasks. Under suitable conditions, this can be interpreted as a marker for prioritization of one task – the one that suffers less – over the other. One source of such task prioritization is based on the use of different effector systems (e.g., oculomotor system, vocal tract, limbs) and their characteristics. The present work explores such effector system-based task prioritization by examining to which extent associated effector systems determine which task is processed with higher priority in multitasking situations. Thus, three different paradigms are used, namely the simultaneous (stimulus) onset paradigm, the psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm, and the task switching paradigm. These paradigms invoke situations in which two (in the present studies basic spatial decision) tasks are a) initiated at exactly the same time, b) initiated with a short varying temporal distance (but still temporally overlapping), or c) in which tasks alternate randomly (without temporal overlap). The results allow for three major conclusions: 1. The assumption of effector system-based task prioritization according to an ordinal pattern (oculomotor > pedal > vocal > manual, indicating decreasing prioritization) is supported by the observed data in the simultaneous onset paradigm. This data pattern cannot be explained by a rigid “first come, first served” task scheduling principle. 2. The data from the PRP paradigm confirmed the assumption of vocal-over-manual prioritization and showed that classic PRP effects (as a marker for task order-based prioritization) can be modulated by effector system characteristics. 3. The mere cognitive representation of task sets (that must be held active to switch between them) differing in effector systems without an actual temporal overlap in task processing, however, is not sufficient to elicit the same effector system prioritization phenomena observed for overlapping tasks. In summary, the insights obtained by the present work support the assumptions of parallel central task processing and resource sharing among tasks, as opposed to exclusively serial processing of central processing stages. Moreover, they indicate that effector systems are a crucial factor in multitasking and suggest an integration of corresponding weighting parameters in existing dual-task control frameworks. N2 - Das gleichzeitige Bearbeiten von mehreren Aufgaben (Multitasking) führt in der Regel zu schlechterer Performanz, zum Beispiel bezüglich Geschwindigkeit und Genauigkeit der Aufgabenausführung. Diese sogenannten Doppelaufgaben- (oder Multitasking-) Kosten sind oft asymmetrisch auf die involvierten Aufgaben verteilt. Dies kann unter bestimmten Gegebenheiten als Priorisierung von jenen Aufgaben, die mit geringeren Kosten assoziiert sind über jene, die stärker durch die Doppelaufgabensituation leiden, interpretiert werden. Eine Quelle für solch eine Aufgabenpriorisierung sind unterschiedliche Effektorsysteme (z.B. Blickbewegungsapparat, Extremitäten, Vokaltrakt), mit denen die Aufgaben jeweils ausgeführt werden sollen. Die vorliegende Arbeit untersucht solche effektorsystembasierte Priorisierung, das heißt, inwiefern assoziierte Effektorsysteme determinieren, ob Aufgaben in Multitasking-Situationen priorisiert verarbeitet werden. Dazu wurden drei verschiedene experimentelle Paradigmen genutzt: a) das „Simultane Stimulus-Darbietungs-Paradigma“, b) das „Psychologische Refraktärperioden-Paradigma“ und c) das „Aufgabenwechsel-Paradigma“. Innerhalb dieser Paradigmen werden Reaktionen (Reaktionszeiten und Fehlerraten) gemessen und zwischen verschiedenen Effektorsystemen verglichen, die a) zum genau gleichen Zeitpunkt gestartet werden, b) mit einem kurzen, variierten zeitlichen Versatz gestartet werden, aber in ihrer Ausführung überlappen, oder c) zwischen denen in unvorhersehbarer Reihenfolge hin und her gewechselt werden soll. Entsprechend dieser drei Ansätze erlauben die Ergebnisse drei wichtige Schlussfolgerungen: 1. Unter simultanem Einsetzen der Aufgabenverarbeitung (und damit ohne extern suggerierte Reihenfolge) folgen Doppelaufgabenkontrollprozesse einem ordinalen Priorisierungsmuster auf Basis der mit den Aufgaben assoziierten Effektorsysteme in der Reihenfolge: okulomotorisch > pedal > vokal > manuell (im Sinne einer absteigenden Priorisierung). Dieses Muster ist nicht durch Bearbeitungsgeschwindigkeit im Sinne eines „wer zuerst kommt, mahlt zuerst“-Prinzips erklärbar. 2. Eine Aufgabenpriorisierung, die auf einer externen Aufgabenreihenfolge basiert (gemessen im PRP-Effekt), kann durch die mit den Aufgaben assoziierten Effektorsysteme moduliert werden. 3. Systematische effektorsystembasierte Aufgabenpriorisierung ist nur dann konsistent zu beobachten, wenn Teile der Aufgabenverarbeitung zeitlich überlappen. Eine rein mentale Repräsentation einer Aufgabe, die in einem anderen Effektorsystem ausgeführt werden soll, reicht nicht dazu aus, um das oben beschriebene Priorisierungsmuster vollständig zu instanziieren. Alles in allem sprechen die Ergebnisse der vorliegenden Arbeit für parallele (und gegen ausschließlich serielle) Reaktionsauswahlprozesse und dafür, dass limitierte kognitive Ressourcen zwischen Aufgaben aufgeteilt werden. Außerdem zeigen die vorliegenden Ergebnisse den substantiellen Einfluss von Effektorsystemen auf Ressourcenzuweisungsprozesse in Mehrfachaufgabensituationen und legen nahe, entsprechende Gewichtungsparameter in bestehende Modelle zu Doppelaufgabenkontrolle zu integrieren. KW - Mehrfachtätigkeit KW - task prioritization KW - response modalities KW - cognitive control KW - Multitasking KW - Effektorsysteme Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-201084 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Eidel, Matthias A1 - Kübler, Andrea T1 - Wheelchair Control in a Virtual Environment by Healthy Participants Using a P300-BCI Based on Tactile Stimulation: Training Effects and Usability JF - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience N2 - Tactile stimulation is less frequently used than visual for brain-computer interface (BCI) control, partly because of limitations in speed and accuracy. Non-visual BCI paradigms, however, may be required for patients who struggle with vision dependent BCIs because of a loss of gaze control. With the present study, we attempted to replicate earlier results by Herweg et al. (2016), with several minor adjustments and a focus on training effects and usability. We invited 16 healthy participants and trained them with a 4-class tactile P300-based BCI in five sessions. Their main task was to navigate a virtual wheelchair through a 3D apartment using the BCI. We found significant training effects on information transfer rate (ITR), which increased from a mean of 3.10–9.50 bits/min. Further, both online and offline accuracies significantly increased with training from 65% to 86% and 70% to 95%, respectively. We found only a descriptive increase of P300 amplitudes at Fz and Cz with training. Furthermore, we report subjective data from questionnaires, which indicated a relatively high workload and moderate to high satisfaction. Although our participants have not achieved the same high performance as in the Herweg et al. (2016) study, we provide evidence for training effects on performance with a tactile BCI and confirm the feasibility of the paradigm. KW - brain-computer interface (BCI) KW - event-related-potential (ERP) KW - P300 KW - tactile KW - wheelchair control KW - tactually evoked potentials KW - replication Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-207775 SN - 1662-5161 VL - 14 ER - TY - INPR A1 - Brych, Mareike A1 - Händel, Barbara T1 - Disentangling top-down and bottom-up influences on blinks in the visual and auditory domain T2 - International Journal of Psychophysiology N2 - Sensory input as well as cognitive factors can drive the modulation of blinking. Our aim was to dissociate sensory driven bottom-up from cognitive top-down influences on blinking behavior and compare these influences between the auditory and the visual domain. Using an oddball paradigm, we found a significant pre-stimulus decrease in blink probability for visual input compared to auditory input. Sensory input further led to an early post-stimulus blink increase in both modalities if a task demanded attention to the input. Only visual input caused a pronounced early increase without a task. In case of a target or the omission of a stimulus (as compared to standard input), an additional late increase in blink rate was found in the auditory and visual domain. This suggests that blink modulation must be based on the interpretation of the input, but does not need any sensory input at all to occur. Our results show a complex modulation of blinking based on top-down factors such as prediction and attention in addition to sensory-based influences. The magnitude of the modulation is mainly influenced by general attentional demands, while the latency of this modulation allows to dissociate general from specific top-down influences that are independent of the sensory domain. KW - eye blinks KW - attention KW - auditory KW - visual KW - visual domain KW - auditory domain KW - oddball Y1 - 2020 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-246590 SN - 1872-7697 SN - 0167-8760 N1 - accepted version ER - TY - THES A1 - Huestegge, Sujata Maya T1 - Cognitive mechanisms of voice processing T1 - Kognitive Verarbeitung von Stimminformation N2 - The present thesis addresses cognitive processing of voice information. Based on general theoretical concepts regarding mental processes it will differentiate between modular, abstract information processing approaches to cognition and interactive, embodied ideas of mental processing. These general concepts will then be transferred to the context of processing voice-related information in the context of parallel face-related processing streams. One central issue here is whether and to what extent cognitive voice processing can occur independently, that is, encapsulated from the simultaneous processing of visual person-related information (and vice versa). In Study 1 (Huestegge & Raettig, in press), participants are presented with audio-visual stimuli displaying faces uttering digits. Audiovisual gender congruency was manipulated: There were male and female faces, each uttering digits with either a male or female voice (all stimuli were AV- synchronized). Participants were asked to categorize the gender of either the face or the voice by pressing one of two keys in each trial. A central result was that audio-visual gender congruency affected performance: Incongruent stimuli were categorized slower and more error-prone, suggesting a strong cross-modal interaction of the underlying visual and auditory processing routes. Additionally, the effect of incongruent visual information on auditory classification was stronger than the effect of incongruent auditory information on visual categorization, suggesting visual dominance over auditory processing in the context of gender classification. A gender congruency effect was also present under high cognitive load. Study 2 (Huestegge, Raettig, & Huestegge, in press) utilized the same (gender-congruent and -incongruent) stimuli, but different tasks for the participants, namely categorizing the spoken digits (into odd/even or smaller/larger than 5). This should effectively direct attention away from gender information, which was no longer task-relevant. Nevertheless, congruency effects were still observed in this study. This suggests a relatively automatic processing of cross-modal gender information, which eventually affects basic speech-based information processing. Study 3 (Huestegge, subm.) focused on the ability of participants to match unfamiliar voices to (either static or dynamic) faces. One result was that participants were indeed able to match voices to faces. Moreover, there was no evidence for any performance increase when dynamic (vs. mere static) faces had to be matched to concurrent voices. The results support the idea that common person-related source information affects both vocal and facial features, and implicit corresponding knowledge appears to be used by participants to successfully complete face-voice matching. Taken together, the three studies (Huestegge, subm.; Huestegge & Raettig, in press; Huestegge et al., in press) provided information to further develop current theories of voice processing (in the context of face processing). On a general level, the results of all three studies are not in line with an abstract, modular view of cognition, but rather lend further support to interactive, embodied accounts of mental processing. N2 - Die vorliegende Dissertation thematisiert die kognitive Verarbeitung von Stimminformation. Basierend auf allgemeinen theoretischen Vorstellungen zu mentalen Prozessen wird zunächst unterschieden in modulare, abstrakte Informationsverarbeitungsansätze und interaktive, verkörperte Vorstellungen kognitiver Prozesse. Diese allgemeinen Vorstellungen werden dann am Beispiel der Verarbeitung von Stimminformation im Kontext der parallel dazu ablaufenden Gesichterverarbeitung konkretisiert. Es geht also u.a. darum, inwiefern kognitive Stimmverarbeitung unbeeinflusst von der gleichzeitigen Verarbeitung von visueller Personeninformation ablaufen kann (und umgekehrt). In Studie 1 (Huestegge & Raettig, in press) werden Probanden audiovisuelle Stimuli dargeboten, bei denen Gesichter Ziffern aussprechen. Manipuliert wird die Geschlechtskongruenz der Stimuli: Es gibt männliche und weibliche Gesichter, die je entweder mit einer männlichen oder weiblichen Stimme synchronisiert wurden. Probanden sollen entweder nur auf die Stimme oder nur auf das visuelle Gesicht achten und jeweils das Geschlecht per Tastendruck kategorisieren. Dabei stellte sich heraus, dass es für die Kategorisierungsleistung eine Rolle spielt, ob es sich um geschlechts-kongruente oder –inkongruente Stimuli handelt: Letztere wurden langsamer bzw. mit höherer Fehleranfälligkeit kategorisiert, was für eine starke cross-modale Interaktion der zugrundeliegenden visuellen und akustischen Verarbeitungsrouten spricht. Dabei wirkte sich inkongruente visuelle Information stärker auf die Stimmbeurteilung aus als inkongruente Stimminformation auf die visuelle Beurteilung, was auf eine Dominanz visueller gegenüber akustischer Informationsverarbeitung hindeutet. Unter starker kognitiver Belastung konnte ebenfalls ein Kongruenzeffekt nachgewiesen werden. In Studie 2 (Huestegge, Raettig, & Huestegge, in press) wurde dasselbe Stimulusmaterial verwendet, aber kategorisiert werden sollten nun die gesprochenen Ziffern (z.B. in gerade/ungerade oder größer/kleiner 5). Damit ist in der Instruktion die Aufmerksamkeit von der Geschlechtsdimension weggelenkt. Dennoch fanden sich auch hier Geschlechtskongruenzeffekte auf die Ziffernkategorisierung, was für eine relativ automatische Verarbeitung von cross-modaler Geschlechtsinformation spricht, die sich dann auch auf die Sprachverarbeitung auswirken kann. In Studie 3 (Huestegge, subm.) wurde die Fähigkeit von Probanden untersucht, von einer Stimme auf das zugehörige (statisch oder dynamisch) dargebotene Gesicht zu schließen. Dies gelang den Probanden in überzufälliger Weise. Weiterhin konnte keine Evidenz dafür gefunden werden, dass bewegte (dynamische) Gesichter besser den Stimmen zugeordnet werden konnten als statische Gesichter. Die Ergebnisse sprechen dafür, dass gemeinsame Quellinformation sich sowohl auf Stimme wie Gesichtsmerkmale auswirkt, und dass implizites Wissen hierüber von den Probanden genutzt wird, um Stimmen Gesichtern zuzuordnen. Insgesamt konnten die Ergebnisse der drei Studien (Huestegge, subm.; Huestegge & Raettig, in press; Huestegge et al., in press) dazu beitragen, bestehende Theorien der Stimm- und Gesichterverarbeitung entscheidend weiterzuentwickeln. Die Ergebnisse sind allgemein eher im Einklang mit einer stark interaktiven, verkörperten Sicht auf kognitive Prozesse, weniger mit einer modular-abstrakten Informationsverarbeitungsperspektive. KW - Stimme KW - Gesicht KW - Voice Processing KW - Stimmverarbeitung KW - Face Voice Matching KW - Informationsverarbeitung KW - Kognition Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-186086 ER - TY - THES A1 - Rubo, Marius T1 - Social Attention in the Laboratory, in Real Life and in Virtual Reality T1 - Soziale Aufmerksamkeit im Labor, in vivo und in virtueller Realität N2 - Social attention is a ubiquitous, but also enigmatic and sometimes elusive phenomenon. We direct our gaze at other human beings to see what they are doing and to guess their intentions, but we may also absorb social events en passant as they unfold in the corner of the eye. We use our gaze as a discrete communication channel, sometimes conveying pieces of information which would be difficult to explicate, but we may also find ourselves avoiding eye-contact with others in moments when self-disclosure is fear-laden. We experience our gaze as the most genuine expression of our will, but research also suggests considerable levels of predictability and automaticity in our gaze behavior. The phenomenon’s complexity has hindered researchers from developing a unified framework which can conclusively accommodate all of its aspects, or from even agreeing on the most promising research methodologies. The present work follows a multi-methods approach, taking on several aspects of the phenomenon from various directions. Participants in study 1 viewed dynamic social scenes on a computer screen. Here, low-level physical saliency (i.e. color, contrast, or motion) and human heads both attracted gaze to a similar extent, providing a comparison of two vastly different classes of gaze predictors in direct juxtaposition. In study 2, participants with varying degrees of social anxiety walked in a public train station while their eye movements were tracked. With increasing levels of social anxiety, participants showed a relative avoidance of gaze at near compared to distant people. When replicating the experiment in a laboratory situation with a matched participant group, social anxiety did not modulate gaze behavior, fueling the debate around appropriate experimental designs in the field. Study 3 employed virtual reality (VR) to investigate social gaze in a complex and immersive, but still highly controlled situation. In this situation, participants exhibited a gaze behavior which may be more typical for real-life compared to laboratory situations as they avoided gaze contact with a virtual conspecific unless she gazed at them. This study provided important insights into gaze behavior in virtual social situations, helping to better estimate the possible benefits of this new research approach. Throughout all three experiments, participants showed consistent inter-individual differences in their gaze behavior. However, the present work could not resolve if these differences are linked to psychologically meaningful traits or if they instead have an epiphenomenal character. N2 - Soziale Aufmerksamkeit ist ein allgegenwärtiges, aber auch ein rätselhaftes Phänomen, das mitunter schwierig zu fassen ist. Wir richten unseren Blick auf andere Menschen, um ihr Tun zu verfolgen und um ihre Absichten einzuschätzen, aber manchmal verfolgen wir soziale Ereignisse auch ganz beiläufig aus dem Augenwinkel heraus. Wir setzen unseren Blick als ein eigenes Kommunikationsmedium ein und übertragen mit ihm teilweise Botschaften, die nur schwer zu beschreiben sind, aber wir weichen mitunter dem Blickkontakt mit anderen auch aus, wenn wir Angst davor haben, zu viel von uns preiszugeben. Unser Blick stellt sich für uns als eine ureigene Äußerung unseres Willens dar, aber die Forschung hat auch gezeigt, dass unser Blickverhalten in beträchtlichem Maße vorhersehbar und automatisch abläuft. In der Vielschichtigkeit des Phänomens liegt für Forscher eine Hürde bei dem Versuch, alle seine Aspekte schlüssig in ein umfassendes Bezugssystem einzuordnen, oder sich auch nur auf die vielversprechendsten Forschungsmethoden zu einigen. Die vorliegende Arbeit verbindet den Einsatz unterschiedlicher Methoden, um sich mehreren Aspekten des Phänomens aus verschiedenen Blickrichtungen zu nähern. Die Versuchspersonen in Studie 1 sahen dynamische soziale Szenen, die ihnen auf einem Computerbildschirm dargeboten wurden. Hierbei wurde ihr Blick in ähnlichem Maße von physikalischer Salienz (z.B. Farbe, Kontrast oder Bewegung) angezogen wie von menschlichen Köpfen, wodurch zwei ganz unterschiedliche Gruppen von Prädiktoren für Blickverhalten in direkter Gegenüberstellung verglichen wurden. In Studie 2 bewegten sich Versuchspersonen mit unterschiedlich ausgeprägter sozialer Ängstlichkeit zu Fuß in einem öffentlichen Bahnhof, während ihre Augenbewegungen erfasst wurden. Mit zunehmender sozialer Ängstlichkeit neigten Versuchspersonen dazu, nahe Personen im Gegensatz entfernteren Personen im Verhältnis weniger anzuschauen. Als das Experiment mit einer gematchten Gruppe von Versuchspersonen in einer Laborsituation wiederholt wurde, zeigte sich kein Einfluss der sozialen Ängstlichkeit auf das Blickverhalten, was der Diskussion um angemessene experimentelle Designs in diesem Forschungsbereich einen weiteren Impuls verlieh. In Studie 3 wurde Virtuelle Realität (VR) eingesetzt, um das Blickverhalten in einer komplexen und immersiven, aber dennoch streng kontrollierten Umgebung zu untersuchen. In dieser Situation zeigten Probanden ein Blickverhalten, das eher dem in echten Situationen als dem im Labor entspricht, indem sie direkten Blickkontakt mit einer virtuellen Person mieden, so lange diese sie nicht anschaute. Durch diese Studie konnten wichtige Erkenntnisse über das Blickverhalten in sozialen virtuellen Situationen gewonnen werden, wodurch der mögliche Nutzen dieses neuen Forschungsansatzes besser beurteilt werden kann. In allen drei Experimenten zeigten Versuchspersonen konsistente inter-individuelle Unterschiede in ihrem Blickverhalten. Es konnte jedoch im Rahmen der vorliegenden Arbeit nicht geklärt werden, ob diese Unterschiede psychologisch bedeutsame Eigenschaften oder eher Epiphänomene darstellen. KW - Aufmerksamkeit KW - Soziale Wahrnehmung KW - social attention KW - eye-tracking KW - Psychologie KW - Virtuelle Realität KW - Soziale Aufmerksamkeit Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-188452 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Rayner, Christopher A1 - Coleman, Jonathan R. I. A1 - Purves, Kirstin L. A1 - Hodsoll, John A1 - Goldsmith, Kimberley A1 - Alpers, Georg W. A1 - Andersson, Evelyn A1 - Arolt, Volker A1 - Boberg, Julia A1 - Bögels, Susan A1 - Creswell, Cathy A1 - Cooper, Peter A1 - Curtis, Charles A1 - Deckert, Jürgen A1 - Domschke, Katharina A1 - El Alaoui, Samir A1 - Fehm, Lydia A1 - Fydrich, Thomas A1 - Gerlach, Alexander L. A1 - Grocholewski, Anja A1 - Hahlweg, Kurt A1 - Hamm, Alfons A1 - Hedman, Erik A1 - Heiervang, Einar R. A1 - Hudson, Jennifer L. A1 - Jöhren, Peter A1 - Keers, Robert A1 - Kircher, Tilo A1 - Lang, Thomas A1 - Lavebratt, Catharina A1 - Lee, Sang-hyuck A1 - Lester, Kathryn J. A1 - Lindefors, Nils A1 - Margraf, Jürgen A1 - Nauta, Maaike A1 - Pané-Farré, Christiane A. A1 - Pauli, Paul A1 - Rapee, Ronald M. A1 - Reif, Andreas A1 - Rief, Winfried A1 - Roberts, Susanna A1 - Schalling, Martin A1 - Schneider, Silvia A1 - Silverman, Wendy K. A1 - Ströhle, Andreas A1 - Teismann, Tobias A1 - Thastum, Mikael A1 - Wannemüller, Andre A1 - Weber, Heike A1 - Wittchen, Hans-Ulrich A1 - Wolf, Christiane A1 - Rück, Christian A1 - Breen, Gerome A1 - Eley, Thalia C. T1 - A genome-wide association meta-analysis of prognostic outcomes following cognitive behavioural therapy in individuals with anxiety and depressive disorders JF - Translational Psychiatry N2 - Major depressive disorder and the anxiety disorders are highly prevalent, disabling and moderately heritable. Depression and anxiety are also highly comorbid and have a strong genetic correlation (r(g) approximate to 1). Cognitive behavioural therapy is a leading evidence-based treatment but has variable outcomes. Currently, there are no strong predictors of outcome. Therapygenetics research aims to identify genetic predictors of prognosis following therapy. We performed genome-wide association meta-analyses of symptoms following cognitive behavioural therapy in adults with anxiety disorders (n = 972), adults with major depressive disorder (n = 832) and children with anxiety disorders (n = 920; meta-analysis n = 2724). We (h(SNP)(2)) and polygenic scoring was used to examine genetic associations between therapy outcomes and psychopathology, personality and estimated the variance in therapy outcomes that could be explained by common genetic variants learning. No single nucleotide polymorphisms were strongly associated with treatment outcomes. No significant estimate of h(SNP)(2) could be obtained, suggesting the heritability of therapy outcome is smaller than our analysis was powered to detect. Polygenic scoring failed to detect genetic overlap between therapy outcome and psychopathology, personality or learning. This study is the largest therapygenetics study to date. Results are consistent with previous, similarly powered genome-wide association studies of complex traits. KW - Human behaviour KW - Personalized medicine KW - Prognostic markers KW - Psychiatric disorders Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-225048 VL - 9 IS - 150 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Rubo, Marius A1 - Gamer, Matthias T1 - Visuo-tactile congruency influences the body schema during full body ownership illusion JF - Consciousness and Cognition N2 - Previous research showed that full body ownership illusions in virtual reality (VR) can be robustly induced by providing congruent visual stimulation, and that congruent tactile experiences provide a dispensable extension to an already established phenomenon. Here we show that visuo-tactile congruency indeed does not add to already high measures for body ownership on explicit measures, but does modulate movement behavior when walking in the laboratory. Specifically, participants who took ownership over a more corpulent virtual body with intact visuo-tactile congruency increased safety distances towards the laboratory's walls compared to participants who experienced the same illusion with deteriorated visuo-tactile congruency. This effect is in line with the body schema more readily adapting to a more corpulent body after receiving congruent tactile information. We conclude that the action-oriented, unconscious body schema relies more heavily on tactile information compared to more explicit aspects of body ownership. KW - Full body ownership illusion KW - Visuo-tactile congruency KW - Body schema KW - Movement behavior Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-227095 VL - 73 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Lange, Bastian A1 - Pauli, Paul T1 - Social anxiety changes the way we move—A social approach-avoidance task in a virtual reality CAVE system JF - PLoS ONE N2 - Investigating approach-avoidance behavior regarding affective stimuli is important in broadening the understanding of one of the most common psychiatric disorders, social anxiety disorder. Many studies in this field rely on approach-avoidance tasks, which mainly assess hand movements, or interpersonal distance measures, which return inconsistent results and lack ecological validity. Therefore, the present study introduces a virtual reality task, looking at avoidance parameters (movement time and speed, distance to social stimulus, gaze behavior) during whole-body movements. These complex movements represent the most ecologically valid form of approach and avoidance behavior. These are at the core of complex and natural social behavior. With this newly developed task, the present study examined whether high socially anxious individuals differ in avoidance behavior when bypassing another person, here virtual humans with neutral and angry facial expressions. Results showed that virtual bystanders displaying angry facial expressions were generally avoided by all participants. In addition, high socially anxious participants generally displayed enhanced avoidance behavior towards virtual people, but no specifically exaggerated avoidance behavior towards virtual people with a negative facial expression. The newly developed virtual reality task proved to be an ecological valid tool for research on complex approach-avoidance behavior in social situations. The first results revealed that whole body approach-avoidance behavior relative to passive bystanders is modulated by their emotional facial expressions and that social anxiety generally amplifies such avoidance. KW - emotions KW - face KW - behavior KW - social anxiety disorder KW - anxiolytics KW - analysis of variance KW - virtual reality KW - questionnaires Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-200528 VL - 14 IS - 12 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Eder, Andreas B. A1 - Dignath, David T1 - Expected value of control and the motivational control of habitual action JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - A hallmark of habitual actions is that, once they are established, they become insensitive to changes in the values of action outcomes. In this article, we review empirical research that examined effects of posttraining changes in outcome values in outcome-selective Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer (PIT) tasks. This review suggests that cue-instigated action tendencies in these tasks are not affected by weak and/or incomplete revaluation procedures (e.g., selective satiety) and substantially disrupted by a strong and complete devaluation of reinforcers. In a second part, we discuss two alternative models of a motivational control of habitual action: a default-interventionist framework and expected value of control theory. It is argued that the default-interventionist framework cannot solve the problem of an infinite regress (i.e., what controls the controller?). In contrast, expected value of control can explain control of habitual actions with local computations and feedback loops without (implicit) references to control homunculi. It is argued that insensitivity to changes in action outcomes is not an intrinsic design feature of habits but, rather, a function of the cognitive system that controls habitual action tendencies. KW - habit KW - outcome devaluation KW - Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer KW - default-interventionist framework KW - expected value of control KW - cognitive control Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-195703 SN - 1664-1078 VL - 10 IS - 1812 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Reicherts, Philipp A1 - Pauli, Paul A1 - Mösler, Camilla A1 - Wieser, Matthias J. T1 - Placebo manipulations reverse pain potentiation by unpleasant affective stimuli JF - Frontiers in Psychiatry N2 - According to the motivational priming hypothesis, unpleasant stimuli activate the motivational defense system, which in turn promotes congruent affective states such as negative emotions and pain. The question arises to what degree this bottom–up impact of emotions on pain is susceptible to a manipulation of top–down-driven expectations. To this end, we investigated whether verbal instructions implying pain potentiation vs. reduction (placebo or nocebo expectations)—later on confirmed by corresponding experiences (placebo or nocebo conditioning)—might alter behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of pain modulation by unpleasant pictures. We compared two groups, which underwent three experimental phases: first, participants were either instructed that watching unpleasant affective pictures would increase pain (nocebo group) or that watching unpleasant pictures would decrease pain (placebo group) relative to neutral pictures. During the following placebo/nocebo-conditioning phase, pictures were presented together with electrical pain stimuli of different intensities, reinforcing the instructions. In the subsequent test phase, all pictures were presented again combined with identical pain stimuli. Electroencephalogram was recorded in order to analyze neurophysiological responses of pain (somatosensory evoked potential) and picture processing [visually evoked late positive potential (LPP)], in addition to pain ratings. In the test phase, ratings of pain stimuli administered while watching unpleasant relative to neutral pictures were significantly higher in the nocebo group, thus confirming the motivational priming effect for pain perception. In the placebo group, this effect was reversed such that unpleasant compared with neutral pictures led to significantly lower pain ratings. Similarly, somatosensory evoked potentials were decreased during unpleasant compared with neutral pictures, in the placebo group only. LPPs of the placebo group failed to discriminate between unpleasant and neutral pictures, while the LPPs of the nocebo group showed a clear differentiation. We conclude that the placebo manipulation already affected the processing of the emotional stimuli and, in consequence, the processing of the pain stimuli. In summary, the study revealed that the modulation of pain by emotions, albeit a reliable and well-established finding, is further tuned by reinforced expectations—known to induce placebo/nocebo effects—which should be addressed in future research and considered in clinical applications. KW - placebo and nocebo effects KW - emotion processing KW - psychological pain modulation KW - late positive potential KW - somatosensory evoked potential Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-201200 VL - 10 IS - 663 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Scherer, Klaus R. A1 - Ellgring, Heiner A1 - Dieckmann, Anja A1 - Unfried, Matthias A1 - Mortillaro, Marcello T1 - Dynamic Facial Expression of Emotion and Observer Inference JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - Research on facial emotion expression has mostly focused on emotion recognition, assuming that a small number of discrete emotions is elicited and expressed via prototypical facial muscle configurations as captured in still photographs. These are expected to be recognized by observers, presumably via template matching. In contrast, appraisal theories of emotion propose a more dynamic approach, suggesting that specific elements of facial expressions are directly produced by the result of certain appraisals and predicting the facial patterns to be expected for certain appraisal configurations. This approach has recently been extended to emotion perception, claiming that observers first infer individual appraisals and only then make categorical emotion judgments based on the estimated appraisal patterns, using inference rules. Here, we report two related studies to empirically investigate the facial action unit configurations that are used by actors to convey specific emotions in short affect bursts and to examine to what extent observers can infer a person's emotions from the predicted facial expression configurations. The results show that (1) professional actors use many of the predicted facial action unit patterns to enact systematically specified appraisal outcomes in a realistic scenario setting, and (2) naïve observers infer the respective emotions based on highly similar facial movement configurations with a degree of accuracy comparable to earlier research findings. Based on estimates of underlying appraisal criteria for the different emotions we conclude that the patterns of facial action units identified in this research correspond largely to prior predictions and encourage further research on appraisal-driven expression and inference. KW - dynamic facial emotion expression KW - emotion recognition KW - emotion enactment KW - affect bursts KW - appraisal theory of emotion expression Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-195853 SN - 1664-1078 VL - 10 IS - 508 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Gromer, Daniel A1 - Reinke, Max A1 - Christner, Isabel A1 - Pauli, Paul T1 - Causal interactive links between presence and fear in virtual reality height exposure JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - Virtual reality plays an increasingly important role in research and therapy of pathological fear. However, the mechanisms how virtual environments elicit and modify fear responses are not yet fully understood. Presence, a psychological construct referring to the ‘sense of being there’ in a virtual environment, is widely assumed to crucially influence the strength of the elicited fear responses, however, causality is still under debate. The present study is the first that experimentally manipulated both variables to unravel the causal link between presence and fear responses. Height-fearful participants (N = 49) were immersed into a virtual height situation and a neutral control situation (fear manipulation) with either high versus low sensory realism (presence manipulation). Ratings of presence and verbal and physiological (skin conductance, heart rate) fear responses were recorded. Results revealed an effect of the fear manipulation on presence, i.e., higher presence ratings in the height situation compared to the neutral control situation, but no effect of the presence manipulation on fear responses. However, the presence ratings during the first exposure to the high quality neutral environment were predictive of later fear responses in the height situation. Our findings support the hypothesis that experiencing emotional responses in a virtual environment leads to a stronger feeling of being there, i.e., increase presence. In contrast, the effects of presence on fear seem to be more complex: on the one hand, increased presence due to the quality of the virtual environment did not influence fear; on the other hand, presence variability that likely stemmed from differences in user characteristics did predict later fear responses. These findings underscore the importance of user characteristics in the emergence of presence. KW - presence KW - fear KW - virtual reality KW - visual realism KW - acrophobia Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-201855 VL - 10 IS - 141 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Lulé, Dorothée A1 - Kübler, Andrea A1 - Ludolph, Albert C. T1 - Ethical principles in patient-centered medical care to support quality of life in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis JF - Frontiers in Neurology N2 - It is one of the primary goals of medical care to secure good quality of life (QoL) while prolonging survival. This is a major challenge in severe medical conditions with a prognosis such as amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS). Further, the definition of QoL and the question whether survival in this severe condition is compatible with a good QoL is a matter of subjective and culture-specific debate. Some people without neurodegenerative conditions believe that physical decline is incompatible with satisfactory QoL. Current data provide extensive evidence that psychosocial adaptation in ALS is possible, indicated by a satisfactory QoL. Thus, there is no fatalistic link of loss of QoL when physical health declines. There are intrinsic and extrinsic factors that have been shown to successfully facilitate and secure QoL in ALS which will be reviewed in the following article following the four ethical principles (1) Beneficence, (2) Non-maleficence, (3) Autonomy and (4) Justice, which are regarded as key elements of patient centered medical care according to Beauchamp and Childress. This is a JPND-funded work to summarize findings of the project NEEDSinALS (www.NEEDSinALS.com) which highlights subjective perspectives and preferences in medical decision making in ALS. KW - ethics KW - quality of life (QoL) KW - care KW - amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) KW - well-being KW - depression KW - coping KW - psychosocial adaptation Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-196239 SN - 1664-2295 VL - 10 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Käthner, Ivo A1 - Bader, Thomas A1 - Pauli, Paul T1 - Heat pain modulation with virtual water during a virtual hand illusion JF - Scientific Reports N2 - Immersive virtual reality is a powerful method to modify the environment and thereby influence experience. The present study used a virtual hand illusion and context manipulation in immersive virtual reality to examine top-down modulation of pain. Participants received painful heat stimuli on their forearm and placed an embodied virtual hand (co-located with their real one) under a virtual water tap, which dispensed virtual water under different experimental conditions. We aimed to induce a temperature illusion by a red, blue or white light suggesting warm, cold or no virtual water. In addition, the sense of agency was manipulated by allowing participants to have high or low control over the virtual hand’s movements. Most participants experienced a thermal sensation in response to the virtual water and associated the blue and red light with cool/cold or warm/hot temperatures, respectively. Importantly, the blue light condition reduced and the red light condition increased pain intensity and unpleasantness, both compared to the control condition. The control manipulation influenced the sense of agency, but did not influence pain ratings. The large effects revealed in our study suggest that context effects within an embodied setting in an immersive virtual environment should be considered within VR based pain therapy. KW - Cognitive neuroscience KW - Neuroscience KW - Psychology Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-202221 VL - 9 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Huestegge, Lynn A1 - Herbort, Oliver A1 - Gosch, Nora A1 - Kunde, Wilfried A1 - Pieczykolan, Aleks T1 - Free-choice saccades and their underlying determinants: explorations of high-level voluntary oculomotor control JF - Journal of Vision N2 - Models of eye-movement control distinguish between different control levels, ranging from automatic (bottom-up, stimulus-driven selection) and automatized (based on well-learned routines) to voluntary (top-down, goal-driven selection, e.g., based on instructions). However, one type of voluntary control has yet only been examined in the manual and not in the oculomotor domain, namely free-choice selection among arbitrary targets, that is, targets that are of equal interest from both a bottom-up and top-down processing perspective. Here, we ask which features of targets (identity- or location-related) are used to determine such oculomotor free-choice behavior. In two experiments, participants executed a saccade to one of four peripheral targets in three different choice conditions: unconstrained free choice, constrained free choice based on target identity (color), and constrained free choice based on target location. The analysis of choice frequencies revealed that unconstrained free-choice selection closely resembled constrained choice based on target location. The results suggest that free-choice oculomotor control is mainly guided by spatial (location-based) target characteristics. We explain these results by assuming that participants tend to avoid less parsimonious recoding of target-identity representations into spatial codes, the latter being a necessary prerequisite to configure oculomotor commands. KW - Adolescent KW - Stimulation KW - Adult KW - Eye Movements/physiology KW - Female KW - Humans KW - Learning/physiology KW - Male KW - Oculomotor Muscles/physiology KW - Photic KW - Choice Behavior/physiology KW - Psychomotor Performance/physiology KW - Saccades/physiology KW - Young Adult KW - eye movement KW - saccades KW - free choice KW - top-down processing KW - bottom-up processing KW - control levels Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-201493 VL - 19 IS - 3 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Suchotzki, Kristina A1 - Kakavand, Aileen A1 - Gamer, Matthias T1 - Validity of the reaction time concealed information test in a prison sample JF - Frontiers in Psychiatry N2 - Detecting whether a suspect possesses incriminating (e.g., crime-related) information can provide valuable decision aids in court. To this means, the Concealed Information Test (CIT) has been developed and is currently applied on a regular basis in Japan. But whereas research has revealed a high validity of the CIT in student and normal populations, research investigating its validity in forensic samples in scarce. This applies even more to the reaction time-based CIT (RT-CIT), where no such research is available so far. The current study tested the application of the RT-CIT for an imaginary mock crime scenario both in a sample of prisoners (n = 27) and a matched control group (n = 25). Results revealed a high validity of the RT-CIT for discriminating between crime-related and crime-unrelated information, visible in medium to very high effect sizes for error rates and reaction times. Interestingly, in accordance with theories that criminal offenders may have worse response inhibition capacities and that response inhibition plays a crucial role in the RT-CIT, CIT-effects in the error rates were even elevated in the prisoners compared to the control group. No support for this hypothesis could, however, be found in reaction time CIT-effects. Also, performance in a standard Stroop task, that was conducted to measure executive functioning, did not differ between both groups and no correlation was found between Stroop task performance and performance in the RT-CIT. Despite frequently raised concerns that the RT-CIT may not be applicable in non-student and forensic populations, our results thereby do suggest that such a use may be possible and that effects seem to be quite large. Future research should build up on these findings by increasing the realism of the crime and interrogation situation and by further investigating the replicability and the theoretical substantiation of increased effects in non-student and forensic samples. KW - concealed information test KW - deception KW - lying KW - reaction times KW - inmates KW - forensic sample Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-177714 VL - 9 IS - 745 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Schneider, Norbert A1 - Huestegge, Lynn T1 - Interaction of oculomotor and manual behavior: evidence from simulated driving in an approach–avoidance steering task JF - Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications N2 - Background While the coordination of oculomotor and manual behavior is essential for driving a car, surprisingly little is known about this interaction, especially in situations requiring a quick steering reaction. In the present study, we analyzed oculomotor gaze and manual steering behavior in approach and avoidance tasks. Three task blocks were implemented within a dynamic simulated driving environment requiring the driver either to steer away from/toward a visual stimulus or to switch between both tasks. Results Task blocks requiring task switches were associated with higher manual response times and increased error rates. Manual response times did not significantly differ depending on whether drivers had to steer away from vs toward a stimulus, whereas oculomotor response times and gaze pattern variability were increased when drivers had to steer away from a stimulus compared to steering toward a stimulus. Conclusion The increased manual response times and error rates in mixed tasks indicate performance costs associated with cognitive flexibility, while the increased oculomotor response times and gaze pattern variability indicate a parsimonious cross-modal action control strategy (avoiding stimulus fixation prior to steering away from it) for the avoidance scenario. Several discrepancies between these results and typical eye–hand interaction patterns in basic laboratory research suggest that the specific goals and complex perceptual affordances associated with driving a vehicle strongly shape cross-modal control of behavior. KW - steering KW - driving simulation KW - gaze control KW - visual orientation Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-200419 VL - 4 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Wallmann-Sperlich, Birgit A1 - Hoffmann, Sophie A1 - Salditt, Anne A1 - Bipp, Tanja A1 - Froboese, Ingo T1 - Moving to an “active” biophilic designed office workplace: a pilot study about the effects on sitting time and sitting habits of office-based workers JF - International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health N2 - Promising initial insights show that offices designed to permit physical activity (PA) may reduce workplace sitting time. Biophilic approaches are intended to introduce natural surroundings into the workplace, and preliminary data show positive effects on stress reduction and elevated productivity within the workplace. The primary aim of this pilot study was to analyze changes in workplace sitting time and self-reported habit strength concerning uninterrupted sitting and PA during work, when relocating from a traditional office setting to “active” biophilic-designed surroundings. The secondary aim was to assess possible changes in work-associated factors such as satisfaction with the office environment, work engagement, and work performance, among office staff. In a pre-post designed field study, we collected data through an online survey on health behavior at work. Twelve participants completed the survey before (one-month pre-relocation, T1) and twice after the office relocation (three months (T2) and seven months post-relocation (T3)). Standing time per day during office hours increased from T1 to T3 by about 40 min per day (p < 0.01). Other outcomes remained unaltered. The results suggest that changing office surroundings to an active-permissive biophilic design increased standing time during working hours. Future larger-scale controlled studies are warranted to investigate the influence of office design on sitting time and work-associated factors during working hours in depth. KW - desk-based KW - office-workers KW - standing KW - online survey KW - walking KW - work engagement KW - habit strength KW - work performance KW - office environment Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-197371 SN - 1660-4601 VL - 16 IS - 9 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Pfister, Roland A1 - Frings, Christian A1 - Moeller, Birte T1 - The Role of Congruency for Distractor-Response Binding: A Caveat JF - Advances in Cognitive Psychologe N2 - Responding in the presence of stimuli leads to an integration of stimulus features and response features into event fles, which can later be retrieved to assist action control. This integration mechanism is not limited to target stimuli, but can also include distractors (distractor-response binding). A recurring research question is which factors determine whether or not distractors are integrated. One suggested candidate factor is target-distractor congruency: Distractor-response binding effects were reported to be stronger for congruent than for incongruent target-distractor pairs. Here, we discuss a general problem with including the factor of congruency in typical analyses used to study distractor-based binding effects. Integrating this factor leads to a confound that may explain any differences between distractor-response binding effects of congruent and incongruent distractors with a simple congruency effect. Simulation data confrmed this argument. We propose to interpret previous data cautiously and discuss potential avenues to circumvent this problem in the future. KW - action control KW - distractor-response binding KW - congruency sequences KW - sequence analysis Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-200265 VL - 15 IS - 2 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Paelecke-Habermann, Yvonne A1 - Paelecke, Marko A1 - Mauth, Juliane A1 - Tschisgale, Juliane A1 - Lindenmeyer, Johannes A1 - Kübler, Andrea T1 - A comparison of implicit and explicit reward learning in low risk alcohol users versus people who binge drink and people with alcohol dependence JF - Addictive Behaviors Reports N2 - Chronic alcohol use leads to specific neurobiological alterations in the dopaminergic brain reward system, which probably are leading to a reward deficiency syndrome in alcohol dependence. The purpose of our study was to examine the effects of such hypothesized neurobiological alterations on the behavioral level, and more precisely on the implicit and explicit reward learning. Alcohol users were classified as dependent drinkers (using the DSM-IV criteria), binge drinkers (using criteria of the USA National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism) or low-risk drinkers (following recommendations of the Scientific board of trustees of the German Health Ministry). The final sample (n = 94) consisted of 36 low-risk alcohol users, 37 binge drinkers and 21 abstinent alcohol dependent patients. Participants were administered a probabilistic implicit reward learning task and an explicit reward- and punishment-based trial-and-error-learning task. Alcohol dependent patients showed a lower performance in implicit and explicit reward learning than low risk drinkers. Binge drinkers learned less than low-risk drinkers in the implicit learning task. The results support the assumption that binge drinking and alcohol dependence are related to a chronic reward deficit. Binge drinking accompanied by implicit reward learning deficits could increase the risk for the development of an alcohol dependence. KW - Alcohol dependence KW - Binge drinking KW - Low risk alcohol use KW - Implicit and explicit reward learning Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-201406 VL - 9 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Lenhard, Alexandra A1 - Lenhard, Wolfgang A1 - Gary, Sebastian T1 - Continuous norming of psychometric tests: A simulation study of parametric and semi-parametric approaches JF - PLoS ONE N2 - Continuous norming methods have seldom been subjected to scientific review. In this simulation study, we compared parametric with semi-parametric continuous norming methods in psychometric tests by constructing a fictitious population model within which a latent ability increases with age across seven age groups. We drew samples of different sizes (n = 50, 75, 100, 150, 250, 500 and 1,000 per age group) and simulated the results of an easy, medium, and difficult test scale based on Item Response Theory (IRT). We subjected the resulting data to different continuous norming methods and compared the data fit under the different test conditions with a representative cross-validation dataset of n = 10,000 per age group. The most significant differences were found in suboptimal (i.e., too easy or too difficult) test scales and in ability levels that were far from the population mean. We discuss the results with regard to the selection of the appropriate modeling techniques in psychometric test construction, the required sample sizes, and the requirement to report appropriate quantitative and qualitative test quality criteria for continuous norming methods in test manuals. KW - statistical models KW - simulation and modeling KW - psychometrics KW - age groups KW - skewness KW - normal distribution KW - polynomials KW - statistical distributions Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-200480 VL - 14 IS - 9 ER - TY - THES A1 - Weller, Lisa T1 - How to not act? Cognitive foundations of intentional nonactions T1 - Wie handelt man nicht? - Kognitive Grundlagen von intentionalen Nichthandlungen N2 - Human actions are generally not determined by external stimuli, but by internal goals and by the urge to evoke desired effects in the environment. To reach these effects, humans typically have to act. But at times, deciding not to act can be better suited or even the only way to reach a desired effect. What mental processes are involved when people decide not to act to reach certain effects? From the outside it may seem that nothing remarkable is happening, because no action can be observed. However, I present three studies which disclose the cognitive processes that control nonactions. The present experiments address situations where people intentionally decide to omit certain actions in order to produce a predictable effect in the environment. These experiments are based on the ideomotor hypothesis, which suggests that bidirectional associations can be formed between actions and the resulting effects. Because of these associations, anticipating the effects can in turn activate the respective action. The results of the present experiments show that associations can be formed between nonactions (i.e., the intentional decision not to act) and the resulting effects. Due to these associations, perceiving the nonaction effects encourages not acting (Exp. 1–3). What is more, planning a nonaction seems to come with an activation of the effects that inevitably follow the nonaction (Exp. 4–5). These results suggest that the ideomotor hypothesis can be expanded to nonactions and that nonactions are cognitively represented in terms of their sensory effects. Furthermore, nonaction effects can elicit a sense of agency (Exp. 6–8). That is, even though people refrain from acting, the resulting nonaction effects are perceived as self-produced effects. In a nutshell, these findings demonstrate that intentional nonactions include specific mechanisms and processes, which are involved, for instance, in effect anticipation and the sense of agency. This means that, while it may seem that nothing remarkable is happening when people decide not to act, complex processes run on the inside, which are also involved in intentional actions. N2 - Menschliches Verhalten ist im Allgemeinen nicht reizbestimmt, sondern zielgerichtet und hat die Absicht gewünschte Effekte in der Umwelt hervorzurufen. Häufig müssen Menschen eine Handlung ausführen, um diese Effekte zu erreichen. Manche Effekte können allerdings besser oder sogar nur dann erreicht werden, wenn man sich entscheidet nicht zu handeln. Welche mentalen Prozesse finden aber statt, wenn Menschen sich entscheiden nicht zu handeln? Oberflächlich betrachtet scheint es als würde nichts weiter Bemerkenswertes ablaufen, da keine Handlung zu beobachten ist. In drei Experimentalreihen zeige ich aber die kognitiven Prozesse auf, die das Nichthandeln kontrollieren. In den vorliegenden Experimenten werden Situationen untersucht, in denen sich Menschen entscheiden nicht zu handeln, um vorhersehbare Effekte zu erzeugen. Die Experimente basieren auf der ideomotorischen Hypothese, die annimmt, dass bidirektionale Assoziationen zwischen Handlungen und den resultierenden Effekten gebildet werden können. Dadurch kann eine Vorstellung der Effekte wiederum die verbundene Handlung hervorrufen. Die Ergebnisse zeigen, dass Assoziationen auch zwischen Nichthandlungen und den daraus resultierenden Effekten gebildet werden können. Diese Assoziationen führen dazu, dass die Wahrnehmung der Effekte selbst die Nichthandlung hervorrufen kann (Exp. 1–3). Außerdem scheint die Planung einer Nichthandlung automatisch eine Vorstellung der assoziierten Effekte zu aktivieren (Exp. 4–5). Diese Befunde legen nahe, dass die ideomotorische Hypothese auch auf Nichthandlungen übertragen werden kann und dass Nichthandlungen kognitiv durch die Effekte, die sie hervorrufen, repräsentiert sind. Darüber hinaus scheinen Menschen ein Verursachungsgefühl (“Sense of Agency”) für die Effekte ihrer Nichthandlungen zu haben (Exp. 6–8). Das bedeutet, dass die resultierenden Effekte (obwohl nicht gehandelt wurde) wie selbsterzeugte Effekte wahrgenommen werden können. Zusammenfassend zeigen die Experimente, dass intentionale Nichthandlungen von spezifischen Mechanismen und Prozessen begleitet werden, die z.B. bei der Effektantizipation und dem Sense of Agency involviert sind. Obwohl es also von außen so scheint, als würde nichts Bemerkenswertes passieren, wenn Menschen intentional nicht handeln, laufen im Inneren komplexe Prozesse ab wie beim intentionalen Handeln. KW - Intention KW - Kognition KW - Ideomotor Theory KW - Sense of Agency KW - Intentional Nonaction KW - Ideomotorik KW - Intentionale Nichthandlung KW - Aktionsforschung KW - Experimentelle Psychologie Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-176678 ER - TY - THES A1 - Adel Abdelrehim Mohamed Soliman, Hadya T1 - Structural Equation Modeling of Factors Influencing EFL Reading comprehension: Comparative study between Egypt and Germany T1 - Strukturgleichungsmodellierung von Faktoren, die das EFL-Leseverständnis beeinflussen: Vergleichende Studie zwischen Ägypten und Deutschland N2 - In most foreign language learning contexts, there are only rare chance for contact with native speakers of the target language. In such a situation, reading plays an important role in language acquisition as well as in gaining cultural information about the target language and its speakers. Previous research indicated that reading in foreign language is a complex process, which is influenced by various linguistic, cognitive and affective factors. The aim of the present study was to test two structural models of the relationship between reading comprehension in native language (L1), English language (L2) reading motivation, metacognitive awareness of L2 reading strategies, and reading comprehension of English as a foreign language among the two samples. Furthermore, the current study aimed to examine the differences between Egyptian and German students in their perceived usage of reading strategies during reading English texts, as well as to explore the pattern of their motivation toward reading English texts. For this purpose, 401 students were recruited from Germany (n=200) and Egypt (n=201) to participate in the current study. In order to have information about metacognitive awareness of reading strategies, a self-report questionnaire (SORS) developed by Moktari and Sheory (2002) was used. While the L2 reading motivation variable, was measured by a reading motivation survey (L2RMQ) which was based on reviewed reading motivation research. In addition, two reading tests were administrated one to measure reading comprehension for native language (German/Arabic) and the other to measure English reading comprehension. To analyze the collected data, descriptive statistics and independent t-tests were performed. In addition, further analysis using structural equation modeling was applied to test the strength of relationships between the variables under study. The results from the current research revealed that L1 reading comprehension, whether in a German or Arabic language, had the strongest relationship with L2 reading comprehension. However, the relationship between L2 intrinsic reading motivation was not proven to be significant in either the German or Egyptian models. On the other hand, the relationship between L2 extrinsic reading motivation, metacognitive awareness of reading strategies, and L2 reading comprehension was only proven significant in the German sample. The discussion of these results along with their pedagogical implications for education and practice will be illustrated in the following study. N2 - In den meisten Kontexten des Fremdsprachenlernens gibt es nur selten eine Chance auf Kontakt mit Muttersprachlern der Zielsprache. In einer solchen Situation spielt das Lesen eine wichtige Rolle beim Spracherwerb sowie bei der Gewinnung kultureller Informationen über die Zielsprache und ihre Sprecher. Frühere Untersuchungen haben gezeigt, dass das Lesen in der Fremdsprache ein komplexer Prozess ist, der von verschiedenen linguistischen, kognitiven und affektiven Faktoren beeinflusst wird. Ziel der vorliegenden Studie war es, zwei Strukturmodelle der Beziehung zwischen Leseverständnis in der Muttersprache (L1), englischer Sprache (L2) Lesemotivation, metakognitivem Bewusstsein für L2-Lesestrategien und Leseverständnis von Englisch als Fremdsprache zwischen den beiden Stichproben zu testen. Zur Analyse der gesammelten Daten wurden deskriptive Statistiken und unabhängige t-Tests durchgeführt. Darüber hinaus wurde eine weitere Analyse mit Hilfe der Strukturgleichungsmodellierung durchgeführt, um die Stärke der Beziehungen zwischen den untersuchten Variablen zu testen. Die Ergebnisse der aktuellen Forschung zeigten, dass das L1-Leseverständnis, ob in deutscher oder arabischer Sprache, die stärkste Beziehung zum L2-Leseverständnis hatte. Der Zusammenhang zwischen L2 intrinsischer Lesemotivation wurde jedoch weder im deutschen noch im ägyptischen Modell nachgewiesen. Andererseits war der Zusammenhang zwischen L2 extrinsischer Lesemotivation, metakognitivem Bewusstsein für Lesestrategien und L2-Leseverständnis nur in der deutschen Stichprobe signifikant. Die Diskussion dieser Ergebnisse sowie ihre pädagogischen Implikationen für Bildung und Praxis werden in der folgenden Studie dargestellt. KW - L1 reading comprehension KW - metacognition KW - L2 reading motivation KW - L2 reading comprehension KW - Leseverstehen Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-186957 ER - TY - THES A1 - Akakpo, Martin Gameli T1 - The influence of learner characteristics on interactions to seek and share information in e-learning: A media psychology perspective T1 - Der Einfluss von Lernendenmerkmale auf die Interaktionen zur Suche und zum Austausch von Informationen im E-Learning: Eine medienpsychologische Perspektive N2 - Research on the deployment and use of technology to assist learning has seen a significant rise over the last decades (Aparicio et al., 2017). The focus on course quality, technology, learning outcome and learner satisfaction in e-learning has led to insufficient attention by researchers to individual characteristics of learners (Cidral et al., 2017 ; Hsu et al., 2013). The current work aims to bridge this gap by investigating characteristics identified by previous works and backed by theory as influential individual differences in e-learning. These learner characteristics have been suggested as motivational factors (Edmunds et al., 2012) in decisions by learners to interact and exchange information (Luo et al., 2017). In this work e-learning is defined as interaction dependent information seeking and sharing enabled by technology. This is primarily approached from a media psychology perspective. The role of learner characteristics namely, beliefs about the source of knowledge (Schommer, 1990), learning styles (Felder & Silverman, 1988), need for affect (Maio & Esses, 2001), need for cognition (Cacioppo & Petty, 1982) and power distance (Hofstede, 1980) on interactions to seek and share information in e-learning are investigated. These investigations were shaped by theory and empirical lessons as briefly mentioned in the next paragraphs. Theoretical support for investigations is derived from the technology acceptance model(TAM) by psychologist Davis (1989) and the hyper-personal model by communication scientist Walther (1996). The TAM was used to describe the influence of learner characteristics on decisions to use e-learning systems (Stantchev et al., 2014). The hyper-personal model described why computer-mediated communication thrives in e-learning (Kaye et al., 2016) and how learners interpret messages exchanged online (Hansen et al., 2015). This theoretical framework was followed by empirical reviews which justified the use of interaction and information seeking-sharing as key components of e-learning as well as the selection of learner characteristics. The reviews provided suggestions for the measurement of variables (Kühl et al., 2014) and the investigation design (Dascalau et al., 2015). Investigations were designed and implemented through surveys and quasi experiments which were used for three preliminary studies and two main studies. Samples were selected from Germany and Ghana with same variables tested in both countries. Hypotheses were tested with interaction and information seeking-sharing as dependent variables while beliefs about the source of knowledge, learning styles, need for affect, need for cognition and power distance were independent variables. Firstly, using analyses of variance, the influence of beliefs about the source of knowledge on interaction choices of learners was supported. Secondly, the role of need for cognition on interaction choices of learners was supported by results from a logistic regression. Thirdly, results from multiple linear regressions backed the influence of need for cognition and power distance on information seeking-sharing behaviour of learners. Fourthly, the relationship between need for affect and need for cognition was supported. The findings may have implications for media psychology research, theories used in this work, research on e-learning, measurement of learner characteristics and the design of e-learning platforms. The findings suggest that, the beliefs learners have about the source of knowledge, their need for cognition and their power distance can influence decisions to interact and seek or share information. The outlook from reviews and findings in this work predicts more research on learner characteristics and a corresponding intensity in the use of e-learning by individuals. It is suggested that future studies investigate the relationship between learner autonomy and power distance. Studies on inter-cultural similarities amongst e-learners in different populations are also suggested. N2 - Forschungsbemühungen zur Bereitstellung und die Nutzung von Technologien zur Unterstützung des Lernens nahm in den letzten Jahrzehnten erheblich zu (Aparicio et al., 2017). Der Fokus auf Kursqualität, Technologie, Lernergebnisse und Zufriedenheit der Lernenden im E-Learning führte dazu, dass die Forschenden den individuellen Eigenschaften der Lernenden nicht genügend Aufmerksamkeit schenkten (Cidral et al., 2017; Hsu et al., 2013). Die vorliegende Arbeit ist bestrebt, diese Lücke zu schließen. Sie untersucht Lernendenmerkmale, die in früheren Arbeiten identifiziert und theoretisch als einflussreiche individuelle Unterschiede beim E-Learning unterstrichen wurden. Diese Eigenschaften des Lernenden wurden als Motivationsfaktoren (Edmunds et al., 2012) in Entscheidungen des Lernenden bei Interaktion mit und zum Austausch von Informationen vorgeschlagen (Luo et al., 2017). In der vorliegenden Arbeit wird E-Learning definiert als Informationssuche und -austausch, der durch Technologie ermöglicht wird und auf Interaktionen basiert. Diese Ideen werden vor allem aus medienpsychologischer Sicht angegangen. Die Rolle der Merkmale des Lernenden, nämlich seine jeweiligen Überzeugungen über die Quelle des Wissens (Schommer, 1990), Lernstile (Felder & Silverman, 1988), Bedürfnis nach Zuwendung (Maio & Esses, 2001), Erkenntnisdrang (Cacioppo & Petty, 1982) und Machtdistanz (Hofstede, 1980) werden bzgl. der Interaktionen, die zur Suche und zum Austausch von Informationen dienen, untersucht. Diese Untersuchungen berücksichtigen theoretische Annahmen und empirische Erkenntnisse, die hier kurz skizziert werden. Das ‚Technology Acceptance Model‘ (TAM) des Psychologen Davis (1989) und das ‚Hyper-Personal Model‘ des Kommunikationswissenschaftlers Walther (1996) liegen den durchgeführten Untersuchungen zugrunde. Mit dem TAM wurde der Einfluss der Eigenschaften eines Lernenden auf Entscheidungen zur Verwendung von E-Learning-Systemen erklärt (Stantchev et al., 2014). Das ‚Hyper-Personal Model‘ skizzierte Ursachen, warum computervermittelte Kommunikation im E-Learning gelingt (Kaye et al., 2016) und wie Lernende online ausgetauschte Nachrichten interpretieren (Hansen et al., 2015). Diesem theoretischen Rahmen folgend, werden empirische Arbeiten umrissen, die die Verwendung von Interaktion, zur Suche und zum Austausch von Informationen als Schlüsselkomponenten des E-Learning beschreiben sowie die Auswahl der zu untersuchenden Eigenschaften der Lernenden rechtfertigten. Aus diesen Arbeiten wurden Ideen für die Messung der Variablen (Kühl et al., 2014) und das Untersuchungsdesign (Dascalau et al., 2015) abgeleitet. Umfragen und Quasi-Experimente wurden hierzu durchgeführt. Diese Instrumente wurden für drei Vorstudien und zwei Hauptstudien verwendet. Probanden wurden aus Deutschland und Ghana ausgewählt, wobei in beiden Ländern die gleichen Variablen getestet wurden. Die Hypothesentestung berücksichtigte Interaktion und Informationssuche und -austausch als abhängige Variablen, während die Überzeugungen bzgl. der Quellen des Wissens, Lernstile, Bedürfnis nach Zuwendung, Erkenntnisdrang und Machtdistanz als unabhängige Variablen dienten. Durchgeführte Varianzanalysen (1.) belegen die Annahme, dass Überzeugungen über die Wissensquelle Einfluss auf die Interaktionswahl der Lernenden haben. Zudem konnte ein Effekt (2.) des Erkenntnisdrangs auf die Wahlentscheidung der Lernenden durch die Ergebnisse einer logistischen Regression unterstützt werden. Des Weiteren (3.) unterstützten die Ergebnisse mehrerer linearer Regressionen den Einfluss des Erkenntnisdrangs und der Machtdistanz auf das Verhalten der Lernenden bezüglich Informationssuche und -austausch. Schließlich (4.) wurde die Wechselbeziehung zwischen Bedürfnis nach Zuwendung und Erkenntnisdrang unterstützt. Die Ergebnisse sind relevant für die medienpsychologische Forschung, Theorien, die in dieser Arbeit verwendet werden, die Untersuchung von E-Learning, die Messung der Merkmale der Lernenden, sowie für die Gestaltung von E-Learning-Plattformen. Die Ergebnisse deuten darauf hin, dass die Überzeugungen der Lernenden über die Wissensquelle, ihr Erkenntnisdrang (NfC) und ihre Machtdistanz, die Entscheidungen, wie sie interagieren und Informationen suchen oder sie auszutauschen, beeinflussen können. Schlußfolgerungen aus der erarbeiteten Theorie und Empirie sowie aus dieser Arbeit befürworten eine stärkere Erforschung der Eigenschaften der Lernenden. Es erscheint darüber hinaus ratsam, dass zukünftige Studien den Zusammenhang zwischen der Autonomie der Lernenden und der Machtdistanz untersuchen. Es werden außerdem weitere Studien zu interkulturellen Ähnlichkeiten zwischen E-Learning-Lernenden in verschiedenen Bevölkerungsgruppen vorgeschlagen. KW - e-learning KW - Individualität KW - E-Learning KW - Media Psychology KW - Interactions KW - Information seeking and sharing KW - information sharing KW - learner characteristics Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-185934 ER - TY - THES A1 - Götz, Felix Johannes T1 - Social Cueing of Numerical Magnitude : Observed Head Orientation Influences Number Processing T1 - Soziale Aufmerksamkeitslenkung von Zahlengröße : Beobachtete Kopforientierung beeinflusst mentale Zahlenverarbeitung N2 - In many parts of the modern world, numbers are used as tools to describe spatial relationships, be it heights, latitudes, or distances. However, this connection goes deeper as a myriad of studies showed that number representations are rooted in space (vertical, horizontal, and/or radial). For instance, numbers were shown to affect spatial perception and, conversely, perceptions or movements in space were shown to affect number estimations. This bidirectional link has already found didactic application in the classroom when children are taught the meaning of numbers. However, our knowledge about the cognitive (and neuropsychological) processes underlying the numerical magnitude operations is still very limited. Several authors indicated that the processing within peripersonal space (i.e. the space surrounding the body in reaching distance) and numerical magnitude operations are functionally equivalent. This assumption has several implications that the present work aims at describing. For instance, vision and visuospatial attention orienting play a prominent role for processing within peripersonal space. Indeed, both neuropsychological and behavioral studies also suggested a similar role of vision and visuospatial attention orienting for number processing. Moreover, social cognition research showed that movements, posture and gestures affect not only the representation of one's own peripersonal space, but also the visuospatial attention behavior of an observer. Against this background, the current work tests the specific implication of the functional equivalence assumption that the spatial attention response to an observed person’s posture should extend to the observer’s numerical magnitude operations. The empirical part of the present work tests the spatial attention response of observers to vertical head postures (with continuing eye contact to the observer) in both perceptual and numerical space. Two experimental series are presented that follow both steps from the observation of another person’s vertical head orientation (within his/her peripersonal space) to the observer’s attention orienting response (Experimental series A) as well as from there to the observer’s magnitude operations with numbers (Experimental Series B). Results show that the observation of a movement from a neutral to a vertical head orientation (Experiment 1) as well as the observation of the vertical head orientation alone (Experiment 3) shifted the observer’s spatial attention in correspondence with the direction information of the observed head (up vs. down). Movement from a vertical to a neutral end position, however, had no effect on the observer's spatial attention orienting response (Experiment 2). Furthermore, following down-tilted head posture (relative to up- or non-tilted head orientation), observers generated smaller numbers in a random number generation task (range 1- 9, Experiment 4), gave smaller estimates to numerical trivia questions (mostly multi-digit numbers, Experiment 5) and chose response keys less frequently in a free choice task that was associated with larger numerical magnitude in a intermixed numerical magnitude task. Experimental Series A served as groundwork for Experimental Series B, as it demonstrated that observing another person’s head orientation indeed triggered the expected directional attention orienting response in the observer. Based on this preliminary work, the results of Experimental Series B lend support to the assumption that numerical magnitude operations are grounded in visuospatial processing of peripersonal space. Thus, the present studies brought together numerical and social cognition as well as peripersonal space research. Moreover, the Empirical Part of the present work provides the basis for elaborating on the role of processing within peripersonal space in terms of Walsh’s (2003, 2013) Theory of Magnitude. In this context, a specification of the Theory of Magnitude was staked out in a processing model that stresses the pivotal role of spatial attention orienting. Implications for mental magnitude operations are discussed. Possible applications in the classroom and beyond are described. N2 - In vielen Teilen der modernen Welt werden Zahlen als Werkzeuge verwendet, um räumliche Zusammenhänge zu beschreiben - seien es Höhen, Breiten oder Entfernungen. Die Verbindung geht jedoch tiefer, denn eine Vielzahl von Studien hat gezeigt, dass Zahlen räumlich in der Vertikalen (bzw. Horizontalen) verankert sind. So können Zahlen die räumliche Wahrnehmung und umgekehrt Wahrnehmungen oder Bewegungen im Raum die Größe von Zahlenschätzungen beeinflussen. Diese Verbindung findet mittlerweile sogar didaktische Anwendung in der Vermittlung von Zahlenbedeutung bei Kindern. Allerdings wissen wir noch wenig über die kognitiven (und neuropsychologischen) Prozesse, die konkreten Zahlengrößenoperationen (d.h. Größer-Kleiner-Urteile, Addition, etc.) zugrunde liegen. Mehrere Autoren deuteten an, dass die Verarbeitung innerhalb des körpernahen Raumes (d.h. des Raumes in Handlungsreichweite) und das Operieren mit Zahlengrößen funktional äquivalent sind. Diese Annahme hat mehrere Implikationen, die die vorliegende Arbeit beschreiben möchte. So spielt die die visuell-sensorische Modalität eine hervorgehobene Rolle bei Verarbeitung wie Orientierung innerhalb des körpernahen Raumes. In der Tat legen neuropsychologische und behaviorale Studien eine ähnliche Rolle des Sehens und insbesondere der visuospatialen Orientierung auch für Zahlengrößenoperationen nahe. Darüber hinaus zeigte die soziale Kognitionsforschung, dass sich Bewegungen, Posen und Gesten nicht nur auf die Repräsentation des eigenen körpernahen Raumes, sondern auch auf das visuospatiale Orientierungsverhalten eines Beobachters auswirken. Vor diesem Hintergrund wird in der vorliegenden Arbeit die spezifische Implikation der funktionalen Äquivalenz getestet, dass sich die visuospatiale Orientierungsreaktion auf die Pose einer beobachteten Person auch auf die Zahlengrößenoperationen des Beobachters erstreckt. Der empirische Teil der vorliegenden Arbeit testet die räumliche Orientierungsreaktion von Beobachtern auf Kopfneigungen in der Vertikalen (bei Augenkontakt zum Beobachter) im wahrgenommenen ebenso wie numerischen Raum. Im Rahmen dieser Untersuchung werden zwei Experimentalreihen vorgestellt, die beide Teilschritte umfassen: Von der Beobachtung der vertikalen Kopfposition (trotz auf die Augen ausgerichteter Blickrichtung) einer anderen Person zur gerichteten Aufmerksamkeitsreaktion beim Beobachter (Experimentalreihe A); sowie von dieser Orientierungsreaktion auf die beobachtete Person zu Größenoperationen mit Zahlen beim Beobachter (Experimentalreihe B). Es zeigte sich, dass die Beobachtung einer Bewegung von einer neutralen Ausgangsposition zu einer vertikalen Kopfposition (Experiment 1) ebenso wie die einer vertikalen Kopfpose alleine (Experiment 3) die räumliche Aufmerksamkeit eines Beobachters in Übereinstimmung mit der beobachteten Kopforientierungsinformation (nach oben vs. unten) verschob. Eine Bewegung von einer vertikalen zu einer neutralen Endposition hingegen zeigte keinen Effekt auf die räumliche Orientierung des Beobachters (Experiment 2). Desweiteren generierten Beobachter bei nach unten geneigten Kopfposen (relativ zu nach oben bzw. nicht geneigten Kopfposen) kleinere Zahlen in einer Zufallszahlengenerierungsaufgabe (zwischen 1 und 9; Experiment 4), gaben kleinere Schätzungen auf numerische Allgemeinwissensfragen ab (überwiegend mehrstellige Zahlen; Experiment 5) und wählten in einem Doppelbelegungsparadigma weniger häufiger diejenige von zwei Antworttasten, die sie in einer alternierend zu bearbeitenden Größenaufgabe für größere Zahlen verwenden sollten. Die Ergebnisse der Experimentalreihe A zeigen, dass die Betrachtung der Kopforientierung einer anderen Person im Beobachter tatsächlich die erwartete gerichtete Aufmerksamkeitsreaktion auslöste. Basierend auf dieser Vorarbeit stützen die Ergebnisse der Experimentalreihe B die Annahme, dass Zahlengrößenoperationen in der visuospatialen Verarbeitung des körpernahen Raumes verankert sind. Damit brachten die vorliegenden Studien Forschung zu numerischer und sozialer Kognition ebenso sowie zur peripersonalen Raumverarbeitung zusammen. Darüber hinaus legt der empirische Teil der vorliegenden Arbeit die Grundlage, um eine mögliche Rolle von Verarbeitung innerhalb des peripersonalem Raumes im Kontext der Größentheorie von Walsh (2003, 2013) auszuarbeiten. In diesem Zusammenhang wird die Größentheorie in einem handlungsbasierten Verarbeitungsmodell spezifiziert, das die zentrale Rolle der räumlichen Aufmerksamkeitsorientierung betont. Implikationen für die Forschung zur mentalen Verarbeitung von Größen und Anwendungsmöglichkeiten im Klassenzimmer sowie darüber hinaus werden diskutiert. KW - Soziale Wahrnehmung KW - Raumwahrnehmung KW - Numerical Cognition KW - Social Cueing KW - Social Cognition KW - Spatial Cognition Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-187161 ER - TY - THES A1 - Flechsenhar, Aleya Felicia T1 - The Ubiquity of Social Attention – a Detailed Investigation of the Underlying Mechanisms T1 - Die Allgegenwärtigkeit Sozialer Aufmerksamkeit – eine Detaillierte Erforschung Zugrundeliegender Mechanismen N2 - This dissertation highlights various aspects of basic social attention by choosing versatile approaches to disentangle the precise mechanisms underlying the preference to focus on other human beings. The progressive examination of different social processes contrasted with aspects of previously adopted principles of general attention. Recent research investigating eye movements during free exploration revealed a clear and robust social bias, especially for the faces of depicted human beings in a naturalistic scene. However, free viewing implies a combination of mechanisms, namely automatic attention (bottom-up), goal-driven allocation (top-down), or contextual cues and inquires consideration of overt (open exploration using the eyes) as well as covert orienting (peripheral attention without eye movement). Within the scope of this dissertation, all of these aspects have been disentangled in three studies to provide a thorough investigation of different influences on social attention mechanisms. In the first study (section 2.1), we implemented top-down manipulations targeting non-social features in a social scene to test competing resources. Interestingly, attention towards social aspects prevailed, even though this was detrimental to completing the requirements. Furthermore, the tendency of this bias was evident for overall fixation patterns, as well as fixations occurring directly after stimulus onset, suggesting sustained as well as early preferential processing of social features. Although the introduction of tasks generally changes gaze patterns, our results imply only subtle variance when stimuli are social. Concluding, this experiment indicates that attention towards social aspects remains preferential even in light of top-down demands. The second study (section 2.2) comprised of two separate experiments, one in which we investigated reflexive covert attention and another in which we tested reflexive as well as sustained overt attention for images in which a human being was unilaterally located on either the left or right half of the scene. The first experiment consisted of a modified dot-probe paradigm, in which peripheral probes were presented either congruently on the side of the social aspect, or incongruently on the non-social side. This was based on the assumption that social features would act similar to cues in traditional spatial cueing paradigms, thereby facilitating reaction times for probes presented on the social half as opposed to the non-social half. Indeed, results reflected such congruency effect. The second experiment investigated these reflexive mechanisms by monitoring eye movements and specifying the location of saccades and fixations for short as well as long presentation times. Again, we found the majority of initial saccades to be congruently directed to the social side of the stimulus. Furthermore, we replicated findings for sustained attention processes with highest fixation densities for the head region of the displayed human being. The third study (section 2.3), tackled the other mechanism proposed in the attention dichotomy, the bottom-up influence. Specifically, we reduced the available contextual information of a scene by using a gaze-contingent display, in which only the currently fixated regions would be visible to the viewer, while the remaining image would remain masked. Thereby, participants had to voluntarily change their gaze in order to explore the stimulus. First, results revealed a replication of a social bias in free-viewing displays. Second, the preference to select social features was also evident in gaze-contingent displays. Third, we find higher recurrent gaze patterns for social images compared to non-social ones for both viewing modalities. Taken together, these findings imply a top-down driven preference for social features largely independent of contextual information. Importantly, for all experiments, we took saliency predictions of different computational algorithms into consideration to ensure that the observed social bias was not a result of high physical saliency within these areas. For our second experiment, we even reduced the stimulus set to those images, which yielded lower mean and peak saliency for the side of the stimulus containing the social information, while considering algorithms based on low-level features, as well as pre-trained high-level features incorporated in deep learning algorithms. Our experiments offer new insights into single attentional mechanisms with regard to static social naturalistic scenes and enable a further understanding of basic social processing, contrasting from that of non-social attention. The replicability and consistency of our findings across experiments speaks for a robust effect, attributing social attention an exceptional role within the general attention construct, not only behaviorally, but potentially also on a neuronal level and further allowing implications for clinical populations with impaired social functioning. N2 - Diese Dissertation beschäftigt sich mit verschiedenen Aspekten grundlegender sozialer Aufmerksamkeitsprozesse. Insbesondere werden durch vielseitige Herangehensweisen einzelne Mechanismen untersucht, die der bevorzugten Betrachtung von Menschen zugrunde liegen. Die progressive Untersuchung unterschiedlicher sozialer Vorgänge widerspricht einiger zuvor angenommener Grundlagen allgemeiner Aufmerksamkeitsprozesse. So zeigen beispielsweise Probanden bei freier Betrachtung naturalistischer Bilder eine klare Präferenz für abgebildete Menschen, v.a. deren Gesichter. Allerdings beinhaltet die freie Betrachtung eine Kombination aus mehreren Vorgängen, wie automatische (engl. bottom-up) und zielorientierte, willentliche Aufmerksamkeitslenkung (engl. top-down), als auch den Einfluss von Kontextzusammenhängen. Dies bedingt weiter die Berücksichtigung offener (engl. overt; Exploration mittels Augenbewegungen), als auch verdeckter Aufmerksamkeit (engl. covert; periphere Erkundung ohne Augenbewegungen). Im Rahmen der Dissertation werden alle genannten Aspekte anhand von drei Studien behandelt, wodurch eine sorgfältige Untersuchung verschiedener Einflüsse sozialer Aufmerksamkeitsprozesse erfolgt. In der ersten Studie (Abschnitt 2.1) wurden zielgerichtete Manipulationen in Form von Aufgabenstellungen vorgenommen, welche die Aufmerksamkeit innerhalb einer sozialen Szene spezifisch auf nicht-soziale Reize lenken sollten, um kompetitive Ressourcen von sozialer und zielgerichteter Aufmerksamkeit zu untersuchen. Interessanterweise überwog die Tendenz, soziale Aspekte zu betrachten, obwohl dies nachteilig für das Lösen der Aufgaben war. Diese Neigung erwies sich für die allgemeine Fixationsverteilung als auch für Fixationen, die unmittelbar nach Erscheinen der Stimuli auftraten. Dieser Befund impliziert, dass soziale Reize sowohl bei dauerhaften als auch frühen Aufmerksamkeitsprozessen bevorzugt werden. Obwohl es einen allgemeinen Konsens gibt, dass eine Implementierung von Aufgaben zu verändertem Blickverhalten führt, deuten unsere Ergebnisse lediglich auf subtile Abweichungen hin, wenn die Stimuli sozialer Natur sind. Abschließend indiziert dieses Experiment, dass auch mit steigender Bedeutung anderer top-down Modulationen bevorzugt soziale Aspekte betrachtet werden. Die zweite Studie (Abschnitt 2.2) bestand aus zwei separaten Experimenten, welche verdeckte und offene Aufmerksamkeit auf soziale Reize untersuchten. Hierfür wurden in beiden Studien dieselben Bilder verwendet, in denen ein Mensch unilateral, entweder auf der rechten oder linken Hälfte abgebildet war. Das erste Experiment bestand aus einer modifizierten Variante des Dot-Probe Paradigmas, bei dem periphere Zielreize entweder kongruent auf der Seite des sozialen Stimulus erschienen, oder inkongruent auf der nicht sozialen Seite präsentiert wurden. Diese Zuteilung basierte auf der Annahme, dass soziale Merkmale auf ähnliche Weise fungieren wie Hinweisreize in traditionellen Spatial-Cueing-Paradigmen, indem sie Reaktionszeiten auf Zielobjekte, die auf der sozialen Seite präsentiert werden, beschleunigen. Tatsächlich wiesen unsere Ergebnisse einen solchen Kongruenzeffekt auf. Das zweite Experiment überprüfte die reflexiven Vorgänge durch die Messung von Augenbewegungen mittels Spezifizierung der Sakkadenrichtung und Fixationsdichte für kurze als auch lange Präsentationszeiten. Wiederum stellte sich heraus, dass die Mehrzahl der initialen Sakkaden kongruent zum sozialen Reiz gerichtet waren. Darüber hinaus wurden die Ergebnisse für kontinuierliche Aufmerksamkeitsprozesse durch eine erhöhte Fixationsdichte auf Kopfregionen der abgebildeten Menschen repliziert. Die dritte Studie (Abschnitt 2.3) behandelte den umgekehrten Mechanismus der Aufmerksamkeitsdichotomie, nämlich den bottom-up Einfluss. Durch die Verwendung eines blickkongruenten Paradigmas, konnte der kontextuelle Informationsgehalt der Szenen so reduziert werden, dass nur noch der aktuell betrachtete Bereich sichtbar war, während der Rest des Bildes maskiert blieb. Somit mussten die Teilnehmer willentlich ihren Blick verändern, um die Szene zu erkunden. Erstens zeigten die Ergebnisse eine Replikation des sozialen Bias bei freier Betrachtung. Zweitens scheint die Präferenz, soziale Aspekte zu selektieren, in der blickkongruenten Darstellung bestehen zu bleiben. Drittens zeigte sich ein erhöhtes wiederkehrendes Blickmuster bei sozialen im Vergleich zu nicht sozialen Bildern für beide Betrachtungsmodalitäten. Zusammenfassend implizieren diese Ergebnisse eine zielgerichtete Präferenz für soziale Reize, welche größtenteils kontextunabhängig ist. Hervorzuheben ist auch, dass bei allen Experimenten Salienzprädiktoren verschiedener Algorithmen in Betracht gezogen wurden, um sicher zu stellen, dass die Tendenz soziale Reize zu bevorzugen nicht alleine durch hohe physikalische Salienz in diesen Bereichen bedingt wurde. Insbesondere für die zweite Studie (Abschnitt 2.2) wurden Algorithmen verwendet, die sowohl untergeordnete Merkmale als Prädiktoren integrierten als auch Deep Learning Algorithmen, welche vortrainierte, übergeordnete Merkmale definieren, um Vorhersagen zu treffen. So wurde das verwendete Stimulusmaterial reduziert, so dass nur Bilder mit niedriger mittlerer als auch maximaler Salienz auf der nicht-sozialen Seite analysiert wurden. Diese Experimente geben Aufschluss auf einzelne Aufmerksamkeitsprozesse bei der Betrachtung von statischen, sozialen, naturalistischen Szenen und ermöglichen ein tiefergehendes Verständnis für grundlegende soziale Verarbeitung, welche sich von nicht-sozialer Aufmerksamkeit abhebt. Die Replizierbarkeit und Konsistenz der Experimente implizieren einen robusten Effekt und suggerieren eine gesonderte Rolle der sozialen Aufmerksamkeit innerhalb des allgemeinen Aufmerksamkeitskonstrukts. Dies basiert nicht nur auf Verhaltensparametern, sondern potentiell auch auf neuronaler Ebene und enthält darüber hinaus auch Implikationen für klinische Populationen mit beeinträchtigten sozialen Funktionen. KW - Aufmerksamkeit KW - Soziale Wahrnehmung KW - Mechanisms of Social Attention Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-184528 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Neueder, Dorothea A1 - Andreatta, Marta A1 - Pauli, Paul T1 - Contextual fear conditioning and fear generalization in individuals with panic attacks JF - Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience N2 - Context conditioning is characterized by unpredictable threat and its generalization may constitute risk factors for panic disorder (PD). Therefore, we examined differences between individuals with panic attacks (PA; N = 21) and healthy controls (HC, N = 22) in contextual learning and context generalization using a virtual reality (VR) paradigm. Successful context conditioning was indicated in both groups by higher arousal, anxiety and contingency ratings, and increased startle responses and skin conductance levels (SCLs) in an anxiety context (CTX+) where an aversive unconditioned stimulus (US) occurred unpredictably vs. a safety context (CTX−). PA compared to HC exhibited increased differential responding to CTX+ vs. CTX− and overgeneralization of contextual anxiety on an evaluative verbal level, but not on a physiological level. We conclude that increased contextual conditioning and contextual generalization may constitute risk factors for PD or agoraphobia contributing to the characteristic avoidance of anxiety contexts and withdrawal to safety contexts and that evaluative cognitive process may play a major role. KW - contextual fear conditioning KW - anxiety generalization KW - startle response KW - panic disorder KW - virtual reality Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-201318 VL - 13 ER - TY - THES A1 - Kaß, Christina T1 - Unnecessary Alarms in Driving: The Impact of Discrepancies between Human and Machine Situation Awareness on Drivers’ Perception and Behaviour T1 - Unnötige Warnungen beim Fahren: Der Einfluss von Diskrepanzen zwischen menschlichem und maschinellem Situationsbewusstsein auf Wahrnehmung und Verhalten N2 - Forward Collision Alarms (FCA) intend to signal hazardous traffic situations and the need for an immediate corrective driver response. However, data of naturalistic driving studies revealed that approximately the half of all alarms activated by conventional FCA systems represented unnecessary alarms. In these situations, the alarm activation was correct according to the implemented algorithm, whereas the alarms led to no or only minimal driver responses. Psychological research can make an important contribution to understand drivers’ needs when interacting with driver assistance systems. The overarching objective of this thesis was to gain a systematic understanding of psychological factors and processes that influence drivers’ perceived need for assistance in potential collision situations. To elucidate under which conditions drivers perceive alarms as unnecessary, a theoretical framework of drivers’ subjective alarm evaluation was developed. A further goal was to investigate the impact of unnecessary alarms on drivers’ responses and acceptance. Four driving simulator studies were carried out to examine the outlined research questions. In line with the hypotheses derived from the theoretical framework, the results suggest that drivers’ perceived need for assistance is determined by their retrospective subjective hazard perception. While predictions of conventional FCA systems are exclusively based on physical measurements resulting in a time to collision, human drivers additionally consider their own manoeuvre intentions and those attributed to other road users to anticipate the further course of a potentially critical situation. When drivers anticipate a dissolving outcome of a potential conflict, they perceive the situation as less hazardous than the system. Based on this discrepancy, the system would activate an alarm, while drivers’ perceived need for assistance is low. To sum up, the described factors and processes cause drivers to perceive certain alarms as unnecessary. Although drivers accept unnecessary alarms less than useful alarms, unnecessary alarms do not reduce their overall system acceptance. While unnecessary alarms cause moderate driver responses in the short term, the intensity of responses decrease with multiple exposures to unnecessary alarms. However, overall, effects of unnecessary alarms on drivers’ alarm responses and acceptance seem to be rather uncritical. This thesis provides insights into human factors that explain when FCAs are perceived as unnecessary. These factors might contribute to design FCA systems tailored to drivers’ needs. N2 - Kollisionswarnungen sollen Fahrer auf gefährliche Situationen aufmerksam machen und ihnen die Notwendigkeit einer sofortigen Reaktion signalisieren. Feldstudien zeigten jedoch, dass etwa die Hälfte aller Warnungen, die von herkömmlichen Kollisionswarnsystemen ausgegeben wurden, als unnötig einzustufen sind. Diese Warnungen wurden zwar auf Grundlage des implementierten Algorithmus korrekterweise aktiviert, allerdings führten sie zu keinen oder nur geringen Fahrerreaktionen. Psychologische Forschung kann einen wichtigen Beitrag zum Verständnis des tatsächlichen Assistenzbedarfs der Fahrer im Umgang mit Fahrerassistenzsystemen leisten. Die vorliegende Arbeit untersuchte psychologische Faktoren und Prozesse, die Einfluss auf den wahrgenommenen Assistenzbedarf des Fahrers in potenziellen Kollisionssituationen haben. Um Bedingungen identifizieren zu können, unter denen Fahrer Warnungen als unnötig bewerten, wurde ein theoretisches Rahmenmodell entwickelt. Des Weiteren wurden die Auswirkungen unnötiger Warnungen auf die Reaktionen und die Akzeptanz der Fahrer untersucht. In diesem Rahmen wurden vier Fahrsimulatorstudien durchgeführt. Die Ergebnisse zeigten, dass der wahrgenommene Assistenzbedarf der Fahrer durch ihre subjektive Gefahrenwahrnehmung vorhergesagt wird. Während das System den weiteren Verlauf einer potenziell gefährlichen Situation ausschließlich anhand physikalischer Messungen vorhersagt, berücksichtigen Fahrer zusätzlich ihre eigenen Manöverintentionen und Intentionen, die sie anderen Verkehrsteilnehmern zuschreiben. Wenn Fahrer vorhersagen können, dass sich der potenzielle Konflikt im weiteren Verlauf auflösen wird, bewerten sie die Situation ungefährlicher als das System. Eine solche Diskrepanz führt dazu, dass das System eine Warnung ausgibt, obwohl der Assistenzbedarf des Fahrers gering ist. Dadurch wird die Warnung als unnötig bewertet. Darüber hinaus ist die Akzeptanz für unnötige Warnungen geringer als für nützliche, wobei dies keine Auswirkungen auf die Gesamtakzeptanz eines Kollisionswarnsystems hat. Während Fahrer zunächst moderat auf unnötige Warnungen reagieren, wird die Intensität ihrer Reaktionen mit wiederholtem Erleben unnötiger Warnungen geringer. Insgesamt scheinen die Auswirkungen unnötiger Alarme auf die Alarmreaktionen und die Akzeptanz der Fahrer jedoch eher unkritisch zu sein. Die Ergebnisse erklären, durch welche menschlichen Faktoren Fahrer Kollisionswarnungen als unnötig wahrnehmen. Diese Faktoren können dazu beitragen, Warnungen an den tatsächlichen Assistenzbedarf der Fahrer anzupassen. KW - Fahrerassistenzsystem KW - Zusammenstoß KW - unnecessary alarm KW - anticipation KW - situation awareness KW - need for assistance KW - manoeuvre intention KW - Unnötige Warnung KW - Assistenzbedarf KW - Manöverintention KW - Warnung KW - Verkehrspsychologie KW - Antizipation KW - Situationsbewusstsein Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-192520 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Rösler, Lara A1 - Rubo, Marius A1 - Gamer, Matthias T1 - Artificial faces predict gaze allocation in complex dynamic scenes JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - Both low-level physical saliency and social information, as presented by human heads or bodies, are known to drive gaze behavior in free-viewing tasks. Researchers have previously made use of a great variety of face stimuli, ranging from photographs of real humans to schematic faces, frequently without systematically differentiating between the two. In the current study, we used a Generalized Linear Mixed Model (GLMM) approach to investigate to what extent schematic artificial faces can predict gaze when they are presented alone or in competition with real human faces. Relative differences in predictive power became apparent, while GLMMs suggest substantial effects for real and artificial faces in all conditions. Artificial faces were accordingly less predictive than real human faces but still contributed significantly to gaze allocation. These results help to further our understanding of how social information guides gaze in complex naturalistic scenes. KW - social attention KW - faces KW - physical saliency KW - visual perception KW - naturalistic scenes KW - eye movements Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-193024 SN - 1664-1078 VL - 10 IS - 2877 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Greving, Carla E. A1 - Richter, Tobias T1 - Distributed Learning in the Classroom: Effects of Rereading Schedules Depend on Time of Test JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - Research with adults in laboratory settings has shown that distributed rereading is a beneficial learning strategy but its effects depend on time of test. When learning outcomes are measured immediately after rereading, distributed rereading yields no benefits or even detrimental effects on learning, but the beneficial effects emerge two days later. In a preregistered experiment, the effects of distributed rereading were investigated in a classroom setting with school students. Seventh-graders (N = 191) reread a text either immediately or after 1 week. Learning outcomes were measured after 4 min or 1 week. Participants in the distributed rereading condition reread the text more slowly, predicted their learning success to be lower, and reported a lower on-task focus. At the shorter retention interval, massed rereading outperformed distributed rereading in terms of learning outcomes. Contrary to students in the massed condition, students in the distributed condition showed no forgetting from the short to the long retention interval. As a result, they performed equally well as the students in the massed condition at the longer retention interval. Our results indicate that distributed rereading makes learning more demanding and difficult and leads to higher effort during rereading. Its effects on learning depend on time of test, but no beneficial effects were found, not even at the delayed test. KW - distributed learning KW - spacing effect KW - lag effect KW - retention interval KW - rereading Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-190783 SN - 1664-1078 VL - 9 IS - 2517 ER - TY - INPR A1 - Händel, Barbara A1 - Schölvinck, Marieke T1 - The brain during free movement – what can we learn from the animal model T2 - Brain Research N2 - Animals, just like humans, can freely move. They do so for various important reasons, such as finding food and escaping predators. Observing these behaviors can inform us about the underlying cognitive processes. In addition, while humans can convey complicated information easily through speaking, animals need to move their bodies to communicate. This has prompted many creative solutions by animal neuroscientists to enable studying the brain during movement. In this review, we first summarize how animal researchers record from the brain while an animal is moving, by describing the most common neural recording techniques in animals and how they were adapted to record during movement. We further discuss the challenge of controlling or monitoring sensory input during free movement. However, not only is free movement a necessity to reflect the outcome of certain internal cognitive processes in animals, it is also a fascinating field of research since certain crucial behavioral patterns can only be observed and studied during free movement. Therefore, in a second part of the review, we focus on some key findings in animal research that specifically address the interaction between free movement and brain activity. First, focusing on walking as a fundamental form of free movement, we discuss how important such intentional movements are for understanding processes as diverse as spatial navigation, active sensing, and complex motor planning. Second, we propose the idea of regarding free movement as the expression of a behavioral state. This view can help to understand the general influence of movement on brain function. Together, the technological advancements towards recording from the brain during movement, and the scientific questions asked about the brain engaged in movement, make animal research highly valuable to research into the human “moving brain”. KW - free movement KW - animal research KW - virtual reality KW - recording methods KW - brain activity Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-251406 ET - accepted manuscript ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Gottschalk, Michael G. A1 - Richter, Jan A1 - Ziegler, Christiane A1 - Schiele, Miriam A. A1 - Mann, Julia A1 - Geiger, Maximilian J. A1 - Schartner, Christoph A1 - Homola, György A. A1 - Alpers, Georg W. A1 - Büchel, Christian A1 - Fehm, Lydia A1 - Fydrich, Thomas A1 - Gerlach, Alexander L. A1 - Gloster, Andrew T. A1 - Helbig-Lang, Sylvia A1 - Kalisch, Raffael A1 - Kircher, Tilo A1 - Lang, Thomas A1 - Lonsdorf, Tina B. A1 - Pané-Farré, Christiane A. A1 - Ströhle, Andreas A1 - Weber, Heike A1 - Zwanzger, Peter A1 - Arolt, Volker A1 - Romanos, Marcel A1 - Wittchen, Hans-Ulrich A1 - Hamm, Alfons A1 - Pauli, Paul A1 - Reif, Andreas A1 - Deckert, Jürgen A1 - Neufang, Susanne A1 - Höfler, Michael A1 - Domschke, Katharina T1 - Orexin in the anxiety spectrum: association of a HCRTR1 polymorphism with panic disorder/agoraphobia, CBT treatment response and fear-related intermediate phenotypes JF - Translational Psychiatry N2 - Preclinical studies point to a pivotal role of the orexin 1 (OX1) receptor in arousal and fear learning and therefore suggest the HCRTR1 gene as a prime candidate in panic disorder (PD) with/without agoraphobia (AG), PD/AG treatment response, and PD/AG-related intermediate phenotypes. Here, a multilevel approach was applied to test the non-synonymous HCRTR1 C/T Ile408Val gene variant (rs2271933) for association with PD/AG in two independent case-control samples (total n = 613 cases, 1839 healthy subjects), as an outcome predictor of a six-weeks exposure-based cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) in PD/AG patients (n = 189), as well as with respect to agoraphobic cognitions (ACQ) (n = 483 patients, n = 2382 healthy subjects), fMRI alerting network activation in healthy subjects (n = 94), and a behavioral avoidance task in PD/AG pre- and post-CBT (n = 271). The HCRTR1 rs2271933 T allele was associated with PD/AG in both samples independently, and in their meta-analysis (p = 4.2 × 10−7), particularly in the female subsample (p = 9.8 × 10−9). T allele carriers displayed a significantly poorer CBT outcome (e.g., Hamilton anxiety rating scale: p = 7.5 × 10−4). The T allele count was linked to higher ACQ sores in PD/AG and healthy subjects, decreased inferior frontal gyrus and increased locus coeruleus activation in the alerting network. Finally, the T allele count was associated with increased pre-CBT exposure avoidance and autonomic arousal as well as decreased post-CBT improvement. In sum, the present results provide converging evidence for an involvement of HCRTR1 gene variation in the etiology of PD/AG and PD/AG-related traits as well as treatment response to CBT, supporting future therapeutic approaches targeting the orexin-related arousal system. KW - human behaviour KW - molecular neuroscience KW - personalized medicine KW - predictive markers KW - psychiatric disorders Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-227479 VL - 9 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Mussel, Patrick A1 - Hewig, Johannes T1 - A neural perspective on when and why trait greed comes at the expense of others JF - Scientific Reports N2 - Depending on the point of view, conceptions of greed range from being a desirable and inevitable feature of a well-regulated, well-balanced economy to the root of all evil - radix omnium malorum avaritia (Tim 6.10). Regarding the latter, it has been proposed that greedy individuals strive for obtaining desired goods at all costs. Here, we show that trait greed predicts selfish economic decisions that come at the expense of others in a resource dilemma. This effect was amplified when individuals strived for obtaining real money, as compared to points, and when their revenue was at the expense of another person, as compared to a computer. On the neural level, we show that individuals high, compared to low in trait greed showed a characteristic signature in the EEG, a reduced P3 effect to positive, compared to negative feedback, indicating that they may have a lack of sensitivity to adjust behavior according to positive and negative stimuli from the environment. Brain-behavior relations further confirmed this lack of sensitivity to behavior adjustment as a potential underlying neuro-cognitive mechanism which explains selfish and reckless behavior that may come at the expense of others. KW - human behaviour KW - psychology Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-231652 VL - 9 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Stegmann, Yannik A1 - Reicherts, Philipp A1 - Andreatta, Marta A1 - Pauli, Paul A1 - Wieser, Matthias J. T1 - The effect of trait anxiety on attentional mechanisms in combined context and cue conditioning and extinction learning JF - Scientific Reports N2 - Sensory processing and attention allocation are shaped by threat, but the role of trait-anxiety in sensory processing as a function of threat predictability remains incompletely understood. Therefore, we measured steady-state visual evoked potentials (ssVEPs) as an index of sensory processing of predictable and unpredictable threat cues in 29 low (LA) and 29 high (HA) trait-anxious participants during a modified NPU-paradigm followed by an extinction phase. Three different contextual cues indicated safety (N), predictable (P) or unpredictable threat (U), while foreground cues signalled shocks in the P-condition only. All participants allocated increased attentional resources to the central P-threat cue, replicating previous findings. Importantly, LA individuals exhibited larger ssVEP amplitudes to contextual threat (U and P) than to contextual safety cues, while HA individuals did not differentiate among contextual cues in general. Further, HA exhibited higher aversive ratings of all contexts compared to LA. These results suggest that high trait-anxious individuals might be worse at discriminating contextual threat stimuli and accordingly overestimate the probability and aversiveness of unpredictable threat. These findings support the notion of aberrant sensory processing of unpredictable threat in anxiety disorders, as this processing pattern is already evident in individuals at risk of these disorders. KW - attention KW - fear conditioning Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-239394 VL - 9 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Vaahtoranta, Enni A1 - Lenhart, Jan A1 - Suggate, Sebastian A1 - Lenhard, Wolfgang T1 - Interactive Elaborative Storytelling: Engaging Children as Storytellers to Foster Vocabulary JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - Positive effects of shared reading for children’s language development are boosted by including instruction of word meanings and by increasing interactivity. The effects of engaging children as storytellers on vocabulary development have been less well studied. We developed an approach termed Interactive Elaborative Storytelling (IES), which employs both word-learning techniques and children’s storytelling in a shared-reading setting. To systematically investigate potential benefits of children as storytellers, we contrasted this approach to two experimental groups, an Elaborative Storytelling group employing word-learning techniques but no storytelling by children and a Read-Aloud group, excluding any additional techniques. The study was a 3 × 2 pre-posttest randomized design with 126 preschoolers spanning 1 week. Measured outcomes were receptive and expressive target vocabulary, story memory, and children’s behavior during story sessions. All three experimental groups made comparable gains on target words from pre- to posttest and there was no difference between groups in story memory. However, in the Elaborative Storytelling group, children were the least restless. Findings are discussed in terms of their contribution to optimizing shared reading as a method of fostering language. KW - storytelling KW - shared reading KW - language intervention KW - preschool KW - language development Y1 - 2019 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-232136 VL - 10 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Greving, Sven A1 - Richter, Tobias T1 - Examining the testing effect in university teaching: retrievability and question format matter JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - Review of learned material is crucial for the learning process. One approach that promises to increase the effectiveness of reviewing during learning is to answer questions about the learning content rather than restudying the material (testing effect). This effect is well established in lab experiments. However, existing research in educational contexts has often combined testing with additional didactical measures that hampers the interpretation of testing effects. We aimed to examine the testing effect in its pure form by implementing a minimal intervention design in a university lecture (N = 92). The last 10 min of each lecture session were used for reviewing the lecture content by either answering short-answer questions, multiple-choice questions, or reading summarizing statements about core lecture content. Three unannounced criterial tests measured the retention of learning content at different times (1, 12, and 23 weeks after the last lecture). A positive testing effect emerged for short-answer questions that targeted information that participants could retrieve from memory. This effect was independent of the time of test. The results indicated no testing effect for multiple-choice testing. These results suggest that short-answer testing but not multiple-choice testing may benefit learning in higher education contexts. KW - testing effect KW - university teaching KW - retrieval practice KW - question format KW - educational psychology KW - net testing effect KW - desirable difficulties Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-190802 SN - 1664-1078 VL - 9 ER - TY - THES A1 - Wannagat, Wienke Charlotte T1 - Cognitive Processes of Discourse Comprehension in Children and Adults - Comparisons between Written, Auditory, and Audiovisual Modes of Presentation - T1 - Kognitive Prozesse beim Textverstehen bei Kindern und Erwachsenen - Vergleiche zwischen schriftlicher, auditiver und audiovisueller Darbietung - N2 - In drei Studien wurde untersucht, wie sich unterschiedliche Darbietungsformate (schriftlich, auditiv, audiovisuell (auditiv + Bilder) auf das Verständnis semantisch identischer Inhalte auswirken. Dabei interessierte insbesondere der Entwicklungsverlauf von der ersten Klasse bis zum Erwachsenenalter. Dass sich Bilder förderlich auf die Verständnisleistung auswirken können, gilt als gut untersucht (z.B. Carney & Levin, 2002). Anders als viele bisherige Studien erfassen wir Textverstehen mit impliziten Maßen, die differenziertere Rückschlüsse auf die, gängigen Theorien zufolge, zugrundeliegenden Prozesse zulassen: Textverstehen geht mit der Konstruktion von drei Ebenen mentaler Repräsentationen einher (vgl. Kintsch, 1998). Weiterhin bedeutet erfolgreiches Textverstehen, eine auf lokaler und globaler Ebene kohärente mentale Repräsentation zu konstruieren (z.B. Schnotz & Dutke, 2004). Mit einem Satz-Rekognitionstest (vgl. Schmalhofer & Glavanov, 1986) untersuchten wir, ob sich das Gedächtnis für die Textoberfläche, die Textbasis und das Situationsmodell bei 103 8- und 10-Jährigen zwischen schriftlicher, auditiver und audiovisueller (Studie 1) und bei 106 7-, 9- und 11-Jährigen zwischen auditiver und audiovisueller Darbietung narrativer Texte (Studie 2) unterscheidet. Weiterhin (Studie 3) untersuchten wir mit 155 9- und 11-Jährigen, inwieweit sich die Fähigkeit der Inferenzbildung zur Herstellung lokaler und globaler Kohärenz zwischen schriftlicher, auditiver und audiovisueller Darbietung unterscheidet. Als Indikator dienten die Reaktionszeiten auf Wörter, die mit einem über (global)- oder untergeordneten (lokal) Protagonistenziel assoziiert sind. Insgesamt zeigte sich, dass Schüler bis zu einem Alter von 11 Jahren nicht nur die Textoberfläche besser erinnern, sondern auch besser in der Lage sind ein Situationsmodell zu konstruieren, wenn einem Text Bilder beigefügt sind. Dies zeigte sich sowohl im Vergleich mit auditiver als auch mit schriftlicher Darbietung. Bei Erwachsenen zeigte sich kein Effekt der Darbietungsform. Sowohl 9- als auch 11-Jährigen gelingt außerdem die Herstellung globaler Kohärenz bei audiovisueller Darbietung besser als bei auditiver. Die schriftliche Darbietung zeigte sich im Vergleich zur auditiven sowohl im Hinblick auf lokale als auch auf globale Kohärenz überlegen. N2 - In three studies, we investigated, if and how different modes of presentation - written, auditory, audiovisual (auditory combined with pictures) - affect comprehension of semantically identical materials. Children, beginning from the age of 7, and adults were included into the studies. A vast amount of studies have shown that pictures can facilitate text comprehension (e.g. Carney & Levin, 2002). Other than the majority of these previous studies, we assessed text comprehension with methods that we assume to allow more differentiated insights into the cognitive processes that - according to current theories - underlie text comprehension. Text comprehension involves at least three levels of mental representations (see Kintsch, 1998). Moreover, text comprehension means constructing a locally and globally coherent mental representation of the text content. Using a sentence recognition task (see Schmalhofer & Glavanov, 1986), we examined whether the memory of the text surface, the text base, and the situation model differs between written, auditory, and audiovisual text presentation in a sample of 103 8- and 10-year-olds and adults (Study I), and between auditory and audiovisual text presentation in a sample of 106 7-, 9-, and 11-year-olds (Study II). Furthermore, we examined with 155 9- and 11-year-olds, whether the ability to draw inferences to establish local and global coherence differs between written, auditory, and audiovisual text presentation. These inferences were indicated by reaction times to words associated with a protagonist's super- (global) or subordinate (local) goal. Overall, the results of these three studies taken together, indicate that children up to age 11 do not only have better memory of not only the text surface, but also of the situation model when pictures are added to an auditory text. This effect became apparent in comparison with both auditory and written texts. For the adults, in contrast, we did not find an effect of the presentation mode. Furthermore, both 9- and 11-year-olds were better at establishing global coherence at audiovisual compared to auditory text presentation. Written presentation turned out to be superior to auditory presentation in terms of both local and global coherence. KW - Textverstehen KW - text comprehension KW - picture comprehension KW - mental representation KW - coherence KW - children Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-162515 ER - TY - THES A1 - Wandtner, Bernhard T1 - Non-driving related tasks in highly automated driving - Effects of task characteristics and drivers' self-regulation on take-over performance T1 - Fahrfremde Tätigkeiten beim hochautomatisierten Fahren - Einfluss des Aufgabentyps und der Selbstregulation auf die Übernahmeleistung N2 - The rise of automated driving will fundamentally change our mobility in the near future. This thesis specifically considers the stage of so called highly automated driving (Level 3, SAE International, 2014). At this level, a system carries out vehicle guidance in specific application areas, e.g. on highway roads. The driver can temporarily suspend from monitoring the driving task and might use the time by engaging in so called non-driving related tasks (NDR-tasks). However, the driver is still in charge to resume vehicle control when prompted by the system. This new role of the driver has to be critically examined from a human factors perspective. The main aim of this thesis was to systematically investigate the impact of different NDR-tasks on driver behavior and take-over performance. Wickens’ (2008) architecture of multiple resource theory was chosen as theoretical framework, with the building blocks of multiplicity (task interference due to resource overlap), mental workload (task demands), and aspects of executive control or self-regulation. Specific adaptations and extensions of the theory were discussed to account for the context of NDR-task interactions in highly automated driving. Overall four driving simulator studies were carried out to investigate the role of these theoretical components. Study 1 showed that drivers focused NDR-task engagement on sections of highly automated compared to manual driving. In addition, drivers avoided task engagement prior to predictable take-over situations. These results indicate that self-regulatory behavior, as reported for manual driving, also takes place in the context of highly automated driving. Study 2 specifically addressed the impact of NDR-tasks’ stimulus and response modalities on take-over performance. Results showed that particularly visual-manual tasks with high motoric load (including the need to get rid of a handheld object) had detrimental effects. However, drivers seemed to be aware of task specific distraction in take-over situations and strictly canceled visual-manual tasks compared to a low impairing auditory-vocal task. Study 3 revealed that also the mental demand of NDR-tasks should be considered for drivers’ take-over performance. Finally, different human-machine-interfaces were developed and evaluated in Simulator Study 4. Concepts including an explicit pre-alert (“notification”) clearly supported drivers’ self-regulation and achieved high usability and acceptance ratings. Overall, this thesis indicates that the architecture of multiple resource theory provides a useful framework for research in this field. Practical implications arise regarding the potential legal regulation of NDR-tasks as well as the design of elaborated human-machine-interfaces. N2 - In den nächsten Jahren wird die Fahrzeugautomatisierung stufenweise immer weiter zunehmen. Im Fokus dieser Arbeit steht das Hochautomatisierte Fahren (HAF), bei dem ein System in definierten Anwendungsbereichen, z.B. auf Autobahnen, die Fahraufgabe vollständig übernehmen kann (Level 3; SAE International, 2014). Der Fahrer muss das Verkehrsgeschehen nicht mehr überwachen, jedoch bereit sein, nach Aufforderung durch das System die Fahraufgabe wieder zu übernehmen. Bisherige Forschung legt nahe, dass Fahrer die freigewordene Zeit oftmals zur Beschäftigung mit sog. fahrfremden Tätigkeiten (FFTs) nutzen werden. Die vorliegende Arbeit beschäftigt sich mit den Herausforderungen, die diese neue Rolle des Fahrers mit sich bringt. Der Fokus liegt auf dem Einfluss unterschiedlicher FFTs auf die Übernahmeleistung und der Frage, inwieweit Fahrer den Umgang mit FFTs an die situativen Bedingungen anpassen. Die Theorie der multiplen Ressourcen (Wickens, 2008) wurde dabei als Rahmenmodell gewählt und für den spezifischen Anwendungsfall von HAF-Systemen ausgelegt. In vier Fahrsimulatorstudien wurden die unterschiedlichen Komponenten der Theorie untersucht. Studie 1 beschäftigte sich mit dem Aspekt der Ressourcenallokation (Selbstregulation). Die Ergebnisse zeigten, dass Fahrer die Beschäftigung mit einer prototypischen FFT an die Verfügbarkeit des HAF-Systems anpassten. Die Tätigkeit wurde bevorzugt im HAF und nicht im manuellen Fahrbetrieb durchgeführt und vor Übernahmesituationen wurden weniger Aufgaben neu begonnen. Studie 2 betrachtete den Aspekt der Interferenz zwischen FFT und Fahraufgabe. Die Modalitäten einer FFT wurden dazu systematisch variiert. Dabei zeigte sich, dass insbesondere visuell-manuelle Tätigkeiten mit hoher motorischer Beanspruchung (z.B. ein in der Hand gehaltenes Tablet) die Übernahme erschwerten. Fahrer schienen sich der Ablenkung bewusst zu sein und brachen diese Art von Aufgaben bei der Übernahme eher ab. Studie 3 ergab Hinweise, dass neben den Aufgabenmodalitäten auch kognitive Beanspruchung die Übernahmeleistung beeinträchtigen kann. Studie 4 beschäftigte sich mit der Mensch-Maschine-Schnittstelle (HMI) für HAF-Systeme. Die Ergebnisse ergaben, dass eine explizite Vorankündigung von Übernahmesituationen die Selbstregulation des Fahrers unterstützen kann. Die Arbeit zeigt die Eignung der multiplen Ressourcentheorie als Rahmenmodell für Forschung im Bereich HAF. Praktische Implikationen ergeben sich für mögliche gesetzliche Regelungen über erlaubte Tätigkeiten beim HAF, genauso wie konkrete HMI-Gestaltungsempfehlungen. KW - Autonomes Fahrzeug KW - Fahrerverhalten KW - automated driving KW - human-automation interaction KW - driver behavior KW - driver distraction KW - Automatisiertes Fahren KW - Mensch-Maschine-Interaktion KW - Fahrerablenkung KW - Automation KW - Verkehrspsychologie KW - Mensch-Maschine-Kommunikation Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-173956 ER - TY - THES A1 - von der Mühlen, Sarah T1 - Fostering Students’ Epistemic Competences when Dealing with Scientific Literature T1 - Die Förderung epistemischer Kompetenzen von Studierenden im Umgang mit wissenschaftlicher Literatur N2 - The abilities to comprehend and critically evaluate scientific texts and the various arguments stated in these texts are an important aspect of scientific literacy, but these competences are usually not formally taught to students. Previous research indicates that, although undergraduate students evaluate the claims and evidence they find in scientific documents to some extent, these evaluations usually fail to meet normative standards. In addition, students’ use of source information for evaluation is often insufficient. The rise of the internet and the increased accessibility of information have yielded some additional challenges that highlight the importance of adequate training and instruction.The aim of the present work was to further examine introductory students’ competences to systematically and heuristically evaluate scientific information, to identify relevant strategies that are involved in a successful evaluation, and to use this knowledge to design appropriate interventions for fostering epistemic competences in university students.To this end, a number of computer-based studies, including both quantitative and qualitative data as well as experimental designs, were developed. The first two studies were designed to specify educational needs and to reveal helpful processing strategies that are required in different tasks and situations. Two expert-novice comparisons were developed, whereby the performance of German students of psychology (novices) was compared to the performance of scientists from the domain of psychology (experts) in a number of different tasks, such as systematic plausibility evaluations of informal arguments (Study 1) or heuristic evaluations of the credibility of multiple scientific documents (Study 2). A think-aloud procedure was used to identify specific strategies that were applied in both groups during task completion, and that possibly mediated performance differences between students and scientists. In addition, relationships between different strategies and between strategy use and relevant conceptual knowledge was examined. Based on the results of the expert-novice comparisons, an intervention study, consisting of two training experiments, was constructed to foster some competences that proved to be particularly deficient in the comparisons (Study 3). Study 1 examined introductory students’ abilities to accurately judge the plausibility of informal arguments according to normative standards, to recognise common argumentation fallacies, and to identify different structural components of arguments. The results from Study 1 indicate that many students, compared to scientists, lack relevant knowledge about the structure of arguments, and that normatively accurate evaluations of their plausibility seem to be challenging in this group. Often, common argumentation fallacies were not identified correctly. Importantly, these deficits were partly mediated by differences in strategy use: It was especially difficult for students to pay sufficient attention to the relationship between argument components when forming their judgements. Moreover, they frequently relied on their intuition or opinion as a criterion for evaluation, whereas scientists predominantly determined quality of arguments based on their internal consistency. In addition to students’ evaluation of the plausibility of informal arguments, Study 2 examined introductory students’ competences to evaluate the credibility of multiple scientific texts, and to use source characteristics for evaluation. The results show that students struggled not only to judge the plausibility of arguments correctly, but also to heuristically judge the credibility of science texts, and these deficits were fully mediated by their insufficient use of source information. In contrast, scientists were able to apply different strategies in a flexible manner. When the conditions for evaluation did not allow systematic processing (i.e. time limit), they primarily used source characteristics for their evaluations. However, when systematic evaluations were possible (i.e. no time limit), they used more sophisticated normative criteria for their evaluations, such as paying attention to the internal consistency of arguments (cf. Study 1). Results also showed that students, in contrast to experts, lacked relevant knowledge about different publication types, and this was related to their ability to correctly determine document credibility. The results from the expert-novice comparisons also suggest that the competences assessed in both tasks might develop as a result of a more fundamental form of scientific literacy and discipline expertise. Performances in all tasks were positively related. On the basis of these results, two training experiments were developed that aimed at fostering university students’ competences to understand and evaluate informal arguments (Study 3). Experiment 1 describes an intervention approach in which students were familiarised with the formal structure of arguments based on Toulmin’s (1958) argumentation model. The performance of the experimental group to identify the structural components of this model was compared to the performance of a control group in which speed reading skills were practiced, using a pre-post-follow-up design. Results show that the training was successful for improving the comprehension of more complex arguments and relational aspects between key components in the posttest, compared to the control group. Moreover, an interaction effect was found with study performance. High achieving students with above average grades profited the most from the training intervention. Experiment 2 showed that training in plausibility, normative criteria of argument evaluation, and argumentation fallacies improved students’ abilities to evaluate the plausibility of arguments and, in addition, their competences to recognise structural components of arguments, compared to a speed-reading control group. These results have important implications for education and practice, which will be discussed in detail in this dissertation. N2 - Die Fähigkeit, wissenschaftliche Texte und die darin enthaltenen Argumente zu verstehen und kritisch zu beurteilen, ist ein zentraler Aspekt wissenschaftlicher Grundbildung, wird jedoch in der Schule kaum vermittelt. Obwohl Studierende die Behauptungen und Befunde, denen sie in der wissenschaftlichen Literatur begegnen, zu einem gewissen Grad kritisch bewerten, zeigen verschiedene Forschungsergebnisse, dass sie dies nicht in ausreichendem Maße tun und diese Evaluationen oft nicht den normativen Standards entsprechen. Darüber hinaus nutzen Studierende Quellenmerkmale nur unzureichend zur Beurteilung. Die Entstehung des Internets und die damit verbundene zunehmende Verfügbarkeit von Informationen stellen uns zudem vor einige wichtige Herausforderungen im Umgang mit diversen Informationsquellen und unterstreichen die Relevanz entsprechender Trainings und Förderungsprogramme. Ziel der vorliegenden Arbeit war es, die Kompetenzen beginnender Studierender, wissenschaftliche Informationen heuristisch und systematisch zu bewerten sowie wesentliche Strategien, die für eine erfolgreiche Beurteilung wissenschaftlicher Informationen benötigt werden, weiter zu erforschen und auf dieser Grundlage Interventionen zu entwickeln, um diese Kompetenzen bei Universitätsstudierenden gezielt zu fördern. Dazu wurden mehrere computergestützte Studien entwickelt, die sowohl qualitative, als auch quantitative Daten, sowie experimentelle Untersuchungsdesigns beinhalten. Die ersten beiden Studien wurden konzipiert, um Förderbedarf gezielt zu ermitteln und Verarbeitungsstrategien zu identifizieren, die in verschiedenen Aufgaben und unter verschiedenen Bedingungen hilfreich sind. Dazu wurden zunächst zwei Experten-Novizen-Vergleiche entwickelt, in denen die Leistungen von deutschen Psychologiestudierenden (Noviz(inn)en) in einer Reihe unterschiedlicher Aufgaben, z.B. bei der systematischen Bewertung der Plausibilität informeller Argumente (Studie 1) oder der heuristischen Bewertung der Glaubwürdigkeit multipler wissenschaftlicher Texte (Studie 2), mit den Leistungen von Wissenschaftler(inn)en aus dem Bereich der Psychologie (Expert(inn)en) verglichen wurden. Die Verwendung von Protokollen lauten Denkens diente dazu, die während der Aufgabenbearbeitung verwendeten Strategien, die die Leistungsunterschiede zwischen Studierenden und Wissenschaftler(inn)en möglicherweise mediieren, in beiden Gruppen genau zu erfassen. Darüber hinaus wurde untersucht, inwiefern unterschiedliche Strategien und die Nutzung bestimmter Strategien sowie relevantes konzeptuelles Wissen zusammenhängen. Basierend auf den Ergebnissen der Experten-Novizen-Vergleiche wurde anschließend eine Interventionsstudie, bestehend aus zwei Trainingsexperimenten, entwickelt, um einige Kompetenzen, die sich in den Vergleichen als besonders defizitär erwiesen hatten, gezielt zu fördern (Studie 3). In Studie 1 wurde untersucht, inwiefern beginnende Studierende in der Lage sind, die Plausibilität informeller Argumente normativ angemessen zu beurteilen und gängige Argumentationsfehler zu erkennen, sowie verschiedene strukturelle Bestandteile von Argumenten zu identifizieren. Die Ergebnisse der Studie 1 legen nahe, dass es vielen Studierenden im Vergleich zu Wissenschaftler(inne)n an relevantem Wissen über die Struktur von Argumenten fehlt und die angemessene Bewertung ihrer Plausibilität für viele von ihnen eine große Herausforderung darstellt. Gängige Argumentationsfehler wurden häufig nicht richtig erkannt. Diese Leistungsunterschiede wurden teilweise durch eine unterschiedliche Strategienutzung mediiert: Studierende zeigten große Schwierigkeit darin, Beziehungen zwischen Argumentbestandteilen ausreichend Beachtung zu schenken. Darüber hinaus verließen sie sich bei der Beurteilung häufig auf ihre Intuition oder Meinung zum Textinhalt, während Wissenschaftler(innen) die Qualität der Argumente in erster Linie auf der Grundlage ihrer internen Konsistenz beurteilten. Neben Plausibilitätsbeurteilungen informeller Argumente untersuchte Studie 2 die Kompetenz beginnender Studierender, die Glaubwürdigkeit multipler wissenschaftlicher Texte angemessen zu beurteilen und dabei auch Quellenmerkmale zur Beurteilung heranzuziehen. Die Ergebnisse zeigen, dass es Studierenden nicht nur schwerfiel, die Plausibilität von Argumenten angemessen zu beurteilen, sondern auch die Glaubwürdigkeit wissenschaftlicher Texte heuristisch zu bewerten. Die Defizite auf Studierendenseite wurden dabei vollständig durch eine unzureichende Nutzung von Quellenmerkmalen mediiert. Wissenschaftler(innen) waren dagegen in der Lage, Strategien zur Beurteilung flexibel zu nutzen. Wenn eine systematische Verarbeitung nicht möglich war (Zeitlimit), griffen sie vor allem auf Quellenmerkmale zurück. Wenn eine systematische Verarbeitung jedoch möglich war (kein Zeitlimit), nutzten sie komplexere normative Kriterien zur Beurteilung, wie etwa die Bewertung der internen Konsistenz der Argumente (Vgl. Studie 1). Die Ergebnisse zeigen außerdem, dass es Studierenden an relevantem Wissen über verschiedene Publikationsarten fehlte und diese Schwierigkeiten waren korreliert mit der Fähigkeit, die Glaubwürdigkeit von Texten angemessen zu beurteilen. Die Befunde der Experten-Novizen-Vergleiche liefern zudem Hinweise darauf, dass sich die in den unterschiedlichen Aufgaben erfassten Kompetenzen auf der Basis einer allgemeineren Form der wissenschaftlichen Grundbildung und disziplinären Expertise entwickeln könnten. Die Leistungen in unterschiedlichen Aufgaben waren positiv korreliert. Auf der Grundlage dieser Ergebnisse wurden zwei Trainingsexperimente entwickelt, um die Kompetenzen Studierender in Bezug auf das Verständnis und die kompetente Bewertung informeller Argumente, gezielt zu fördern (Studie 3). Experiment 1 beschreibt einen möglichen Interventionsansatz, um Studierende, basierend auf Toulmins (1958) Argumentationsmodell, besser mit der Struktur von Argumenten vertraut zu machen. Die Leistungen der Versuchsgruppe, verschiedene Argumentbestandteile dieses Modells korrekt zu identifizieren, wurden dabei in einem Prä-Post-Follow-up Design mit den Leistungen einer Kontrollgruppe verglichen, in der die Fähigkeit des schnellen Lesens trainiert wurde. Die Ergebnisse zeigen, dass das Training vor allem für das Verständnis komplexer und weniger typischer Argumente hilfreich war und Elemente, die die Beziehung zwischen verschiedenen Bestandteilen deutlich machten, im Posttest besser verstanden wurden als in einer Kontrollgruppe. Darüber hinaus konnte ein Interaktionseffekt mit der Studienleistung gezeigt werden. Besonders „gute“ Studierende mit hohen Durchschnittsnoten konnten am meisten von diesem Training profitieren. Die Ergebnisse von Experiment 2 zeigten, dass ein Training, in dem das Konzept der Plausibilität, normative Kriterien der Argumentbewertung, sowie Argumentationsfehler vermittelt wurden, die Kompetenzen Studierender, die Plausibilität informeller Argumente normativ angemessen zu beurteilen, im Vergleich mit einer Kontrollgruppe, deutlich verbessern konnte. Die Ergebnisse der genannten Studien liefern wichtige Implikationen für die wissenschaftliche Praxis an den Hochschulen, welche in dieser Arbeit ausführlich diskutiert werden. KW - Textverstehen KW - Wissenschaftliche Literatur KW - Epistemic Competences KW - Higher Education KW - Student KW - Förderung KW - Epistemische Kompetenzen KW - Kompetenzen im Hochschulsektor Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-167343 N1 - Prof. Dr. Tobias Richter supervised this dissertation. Two of the studies reported have been published in international journals, the third study was submitted (see references below). References: von der Mühlen, S., Richter, T., Schmid, S. & Berthold, K. (2017). How to Improve Argumentation Comprehension in University Students: Experimental Tests of Two Training Approaches. Manuskript zur Publikation eingereicht. von der Mühlen, S., Richter, T., Schmid, S., Berthold, K. & Schmidt, E. M. (2016). The use of source-related strategies in evaluating multiple psychology texts: A student-scientist comparison. Reading and Writing, 8, 1677–1698. von der Mühlen, S., Richter, T., Schmid, S., Schmidt, E. M. & Berthold, K. (2016). Judging the plausibility of arguments in scientific texts: A student-scientist comparison. Thinking & Reasoning, 22, 221–246. ER - TY - THES A1 - Krishna, Anand T1 - Regulatory Focus Theory and Information Processing - A Series of Exploratory Studies T1 - Die Theorie des regulatorischen Fokus im Zusammenhang mit Informationsverarbeitung - eine Reihe explorativer Studien N2 - Regulatory focus (RF) theory (Higgins, 1997) states that individuals follow different strategic concerns when focusing on gains (promotion) rather than losses (prevention). Applying the Reflective-Impulsive Model (RIM, Strack & Deutsch, 2004), this dissertation investigates RF’s influence on basic information processing, specifically semantic processing (Study 1), semantic (Study 2) and affective (Study 3) associative priming, and basic reflective operations (Studies 4-7). Study 1 showed no effect of RF on pre-activation of RF-related semantic concepts in a lexical decision task (LDT). Study 2 indicated that primes fitting a promotion focus improve performance in a LDT for chronically promotion-focused individuals, but not chronically prevention-focused individuals. However, the latter performed better when targets fit their focus. Stronger affect and arousal after processing valent words fitting an RF may explain this pattern. Study 3 showed some evidence for stronger priming effects for negative primes in a bona-fide pipeline task (Fazio et al., 1995) for chronically prevention-focused participants, while also providing evidence that situational prevention focus insulates individuals from misattributing the valence of simple primes. Studies 4-7 showed that a strong chronic prevention focus leads to greater negation effects for valent primes in an Affect Misattribution Procedure (Payne et al., 2005), especially when it fits the situation. Furthermore, Study 6 showed that these effects result from stronger weighting of negated valence rather than greater ease in negation. Study 7 showed that the increased negation effect is independent of time pressure. Broad implications are discussed, including how RF effects on basic processing may explain higher-order RF effects. N2 - Die Theorie des regulatorischen Fokus (RF; Higgins, 1997) besagt, dass Individuen sich an unterschiedlichen strategischen Überlegungen orientieren, wenn sie sich auf Gewinne (Annäherung) statt Verluste (Vermeidung) konzentrieren. Unter Anwendung des Reflexiv-Impulsiven Modells (RIM; Strack & Deutsch, 2004) untersucht diese Dissertation den Einfluss des RF auf grundlegende Informationsverarbeitung, insbesondere semantische Verarbeitung (Studie 1), semantische (Studie 2) und affektive (Studie 3) assoziative Bahnung, und grundlegende reflexive Operationen (Studien 4-7). Studie 1 zeigte keinen Effekt des RF auf Präaktivierung von RF-relevanten Konzepten in einer lexikalischen Entscheidungsaufgabe (LEA). Studie 2 deutete darauf hin, dass Bahnungsreize, die zu einem Annäherungsfokus passen, die Leistung in einer LEA bei Individuen mit starkem chronischen Annäherungsfokus, aber nicht bei Individuen mit starkem chronischen Vermeidungsfokus verbessern. Letztere schnitten jedoch besser ab, wenn die Zielreize zu ihrem Fokus passten. Stärkerer Affekt und Anregung nach der Verabeitung valenter Wörter, die zu einem RF passen, könnten dieses Muster erklären. Studie 3 brachte etwas Evidenz dafür, dass negative Bahnungsreize in einer Bona-Fide-Pipeline-Aufgabe (Fazio u.a., 1995) bei Individuen mit chronischem Vermeidungsfokus stärkere Bahnungseffekte produzieren. Gleichzeitig lieferte Studie 3 Belege, dass ein situationaler Vermeidungsfokus davor abschirmt, die Valenz von einfachen Bahnungsreizen fehlzuattribuieren. Studien 4-7 zeigten, dass ein starker chronischer Vermeidungsfokus zu stärkeren Negationseffekten in einer Affekt-Misattributionsprozedur (Payne u.a., 2005) führt, vor allem, wenn er zur Situation passt. Darüber hinaus zeigte Studie 6, dass diese Effekte auf eine stärkere Wertung von negierter Valenz und nicht auf erleichterte Negation zurückzuführen sind. Studie 7 zeigte, dass die Verstärkung der Negation unabhängig von Zeitdruck ist. Breite Implikationen werden diskutiert, unter anderem wie RF-Effekte bei der grundlegenden Informationsverarbeitung RF-Effekte auf höheren Ebenen erklären können. KW - Motivation KW - Sozialpsychologie KW - Informationsverarbeitung KW - Semantic cognition KW - Propositional processing KW - Regulatory focus KW - Semantische Verarbeitung KW - Propositionale Verarbeitung KW - Regulatorischer Fokus KW - Motivationspsychologie KW - Experiment / Sozialpsychologie Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-163365 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Suchotzki, Kristina A1 - Gamer, Matthias T1 - Alcohol facilitates detection of concealed identity information JF - Scientific Reports N2 - The Concealed Information Test (CIT) is a well-validated means to detect whether someone possesses certain (e.g., crime-relevant) information. The current study investigated whether alcohol intoxication during CIT administration influences reaction time (RT) CIT-effects. Two opposing predictions can be made. First, by decreasing attention to critical information, alcohol intoxication could diminish CIT-effects. Second, by hampering the inhibition of truthful responses, alcohol intoxication could increase CIT-effects. A correlational field design was employed. Participants (n = 42) were recruited and tested at a bar, where alcohol consumption was voluntary and incidental. Participants completed a CIT, in which they were instructed to hide knowledge of their true identity. BAC was estimated via breath alcohol ratio. Results revealed that higher BAC levels were correlated with higher CIT-effects. Our results demonstrate that robust CIT effects can be obtained even when testing conditions differ from typical laboratory settings and strengthen the idea that response inhibition contributes to the RT-CIT effect. KW - drug regulation KW - human behaviour KW - alcohol KW - Concealed Information Test KW - reaction time Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-176662 VL - 8 IS - 7825 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Pfister, Roland A1 - Schwarz, Katharina A. T1 - Should we pre-date the beginning of scientific psychology to 1787? JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - No abstract available. KW - psychology KW - history Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-177641 VL - 9 IS - 2481 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Madan, Christopher R. A1 - Bayer, Janine A1 - Gamer, Matthias A1 - Lonsdorf, Tina B. A1 - Sommer, Tobias T1 - Visual Complexity and Affect: Ratings Reflect More Than Meets the Eye JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - Pictorial stimuli can vary on many dimensions, several aspects of which are captured by the term ‘visual complexity.’ Visual complexity can be described as, “a picture of a few objects, colors, or structures would be less complex than a very colorful picture of many objects that is composed of several components.” Prior studies have reported a relationship between affect and visual complexity, where complex pictures are rated as more pleasant and arousing. However, a relationship in the opposite direction, an effect of affect on visual complexity, is also possible; emotional arousal and valence are known to influence selective attention and visual processing. In a series of experiments, we found that ratings of visual complexity correlated with affective ratings, and independently also with computational measures of visual complexity. These computational measures did not correlate with affect, suggesting that complexity ratings are separately related to distinct factors. We investigated the relationship between affect and ratings of visual complexity, finding an ‘arousal-complexity bias’ to be a robust phenomenon. Moreover, we found this bias could be attenuated when explicitly indicated but did not correlate with inter-individual difference measures of affective processing, and was largely unrelated to cognitive and eyetracking measures. Taken together, the arousal-complexity bias seems to be caused by a relationship between arousal and visual processing as it has been described for the greater vividness of arousing pictures. The described arousal-complexity bias is also of relevance from an experimental perspective because visual complexity is often considered a variable to control for when using pictorial stimuli. KW - visual complexity KW - affect KW - arousal KW - valence KW - eyetracking KW - emotion Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-190015 SN - 1664-1078 VL - 8 IS - 2368 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Wolff, Hans-Georg A1 - Weikamp, Julia G. A1 - Batinic, Bernad T1 - Implicit Motives as Determinants of Networking Behaviors JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - In today’s world of work, networking behaviors are an important and viable strategy to enhance success in work and career domains. Concerning personality as an antecedent of networking behaviors, prior studies have exclusively relied on trait perspectives that focus on how people feel, think, and act. Adopting a motivational perspective on personality, we enlarge this focus and argue that beyond traits predominantly tapping social content, motives shed further light on instrumental aspects of networking – or why people network. We use McClelland’s implicit motives framework of need for power (nPow), need for achievement (nAch), and need for affiliation (nAff) to examine instrumental determinants of networking. Using a facet theoretical approach to networking behaviors, we predict differential relations of these three motives with facets of (1) internal vs. external networking and (2) building, maintaining, and using contacts. We conducted an online study, in which we temporally separate measures (N = 539 employed individuals) to examine our hypotheses. Using multivariate latent regression, we show that nAch is related to networking in general. In line with theoretical differences between networking facets, we find that nAff is positively related to building contacts, whereas nPow is positively related to using internal contacts. In sum, this study shows that networking is not only driven by social factors (i.e., nAff), but instead the achievement motive is the most important driver of networking behaviors. KW - networking KW - social capital KW - implicit motives KW - work behavior KW - social interaction KW - career self-management Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-189954 SN - 1664-1078 VL - 9 IS - 411 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Krishna, Anand A1 - Peter, Sebastian M. T1 - Questionable research practices in student final theses – prevalence, attitudes, and the role of the supervisor’s perceived attitudes JF - PLoS ONE N2 - Although questionable research practices (QRPs) and p-hacking have received attention in recent years, little research has focused on their prevalence and acceptance in students. Students are the researchers of the future and will represent the field in the future. Therefore, they should not be learning to use and accept QRPs, which would reduce their ability to produce and evaluate meaningful research. 207 psychology students and fresh graduates provided self-report data on the prevalence and predictors of QRPs. Attitudes towards QRPs, belief that significant results constitute better science or lead to better grades, motivation, and stress levels were predictors. Furthermore, we assessed perceived supervisor attitudes towards QRPs as an important predictive factor. The results were in line with estimates of QRP prevalence from academia. The best predictor of QRP use was students’ QRP attitudes. Perceived supervisor attitudes exerted both a direct and indirect effect via student attitudes. Motivation to write a good thesis was a protective factor, whereas stress had no effect. Students in this sample did not subscribe to beliefs that significant results were better for science or their grades. Such beliefs further did not impact QRP attitudes or use in this sample. Finally, students engaged in more QRPs pertaining to reporting and analysis than those pertaining to study design. We conclude that supervisors have an important function in shaping students’ attitudes towards QRPs and can improve their research practices by motivating them well. Furthermore, this research provides some impetus towards identifying predictors of QRP use in academia. KW - supervisors KW - psychology KW - human learning KW - learning KW - careers KW - scientists KW - psychometrics KW - psychologists Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-177296 VL - 13 IS - 8 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Rubo, Marius A1 - Gamer, Matthias T1 - Social content and emotional valence modulate gaze fixations in dynamic scenes JF - Scientific Reports N2 - Previous research has shown that low-level visual features (i.e., low-level visual saliency) as well as socially relevant information predict gaze allocation in free viewing conditions. However, these studies mainly used static and highly controlled stimulus material, thus revealing little about the robustness of attentional processes across diverging situations. Secondly, the influence of affective stimulus characteristics on visual exploration patterns remains poorly understood. Participants in the present study freely viewed a set of naturalistic, contextually rich video clips from a variety of settings that were capable of eliciting different moods. Using recordings of eye movements, we quantified to what degree social information, emotional valence and low-level visual features influenced gaze allocation using generalized linear mixed models. We found substantial and similarly large regression weights for low-level saliency and social information, affirming the importance of both predictor classes under ecologically more valid dynamic stimulation conditions. Differences in predictor strength between individuals were large and highly stable across videos. Additionally, low-level saliency was less important for fixation selection in videos containing persons than in videos not containing persons, and less important for videos perceived as negative. We discuss the generalizability of these findings and the feasibility of applying this research paradigm to patient groups. KW - Human behaviour KW - Motion detection KW - Social neuroscience Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-227106 VL - 8 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Schiele, Miriam A. A1 - Ziegler, Christiane A1 - Kollert, Leonie A1 - Katzorke, Andrea A1 - Schartner, Christoph A1 - Busch, Yasmin A1 - Gromer, Daniel A1 - Reif, Andreas A1 - Pauli, Paul A1 - Deckert, Jürgen A1 - Herrmann, Martin J. A1 - Domschke, Katharina T1 - Plasticity of Functional MAOA Gene Methylation in Acrophobia JF - International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology N2 - Epigenetic mechanisms have been proposed to mediate fear extinction in animal models. Here, MAOA methylation was analyzed via direct sequencing of sodium bisulfite-treated DNA extracted from blood cells before and after a 2-week exposure therapy in a sample of n = 28 female patients with acrophobia as well as in n = 28 matched healthy female controls. Clinical response was measured using the Acrophobia Questionnaire and the Attitude Towards Heights Questionnaire. The functional relevance of altered MAOA methylation was investigated by luciferase-based reporter gene assays. MAOA methylation was found to be significantly decreased in patients with acrophobia compared with healthy controls. Furthermore, MAOA methylation levels were shown to significantly increase after treatment and correlate with treatment response as reflected by decreasing Acrophobia Questionnaire/Attitude Towards Heights Questionnaire scores. Functional analyses revealed decreased reporter gene activity in presence of methylated compared with unmethylated pCpGfree_MAOA reporter gene vector constructs. The present proof-of-concept psychotherapy-epigenetic study for the first time suggests functional MAOA methylation changes as a potential epigenetic correlate of treatment response in acrophobia and fosters further investigation into the notion of epigenetic mechanisms underlying fear extinction. KW - monoamine oxidase A KW - anxiety KW - extinction KW - epigenetics KW - DNA methylation Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-228571 VL - 21 IS - 9 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Trafimow, David A1 - Amrhein, Valentin A1 - Areshenkoff, Corson N. A1 - Barrera-Causil, Carlos J. A1 - Beh, Eric J. A1 - Bilgiç, Yusuf K. A1 - Bono, Roser A1 - Bradley, Michael T. A1 - Briggs, William M. A1 - Cepeda-Freyre, Héctor A. A1 - Chaigneau, Sergio E. A1 - Ciocca, Daniel R. A1 - Correa, Juan C. A1 - Cousineau, Denis A1 - de Boer, Michiel R. A1 - Dhar, Subhra S. A1 - Dolgov, Igor A1 - Gómez-Benito, Juana A1 - Grendar, Marian A1 - Grice, James W. A1 - Guerrero-Gimenez, Martin E. A1 - Gutiérrez, Andrés A1 - Huedo-Medina, Tania B. A1 - Jaffe, Klaus A1 - Janyan, Armina A1 - Karimnezhad, Ali A1 - Korner-Nievergelt, Fränzi A1 - Kosugi, Koji A1 - Lachmair, Martin A1 - Ledesma, Rubén D. A1 - Limongi, Roberto A1 - Liuzza, Marco T. A1 - Lombardo, Rosaria A1 - Marks, Michael J. A1 - Meinlschmidt, Gunther A1 - Nalborczyk, Ladislas A1 - Nguyen, Hung T. A1 - Ospina, Raydonal A1 - Perezgonzalez, Jose D. A1 - Pfister, Roland A1 - Rahona, Juan J. A1 - Rodríguez-Medina, David A. A1 - Romão, Xavier A1 - Ruiz-Fernández, Susana A1 - Suarez, Isabel A1 - Tegethoff, Marion A1 - Tejo, Mauricio A1 - van de Schoot, Rens A1 - Vankov, Ivan I. A1 - Velasco-Forero, Santiago A1 - Wang, Tonghui A1 - Yamada, Yuki A1 - Zoppino, Felipe C. M. A1 - Marmolejo-Ramos, Fernando T1 - Manipulating the Alpha Level Cannot Cure Significance Testing JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - We argue that making accept/reject decisions on scientific hypotheses, including a recent call for changing the canonical alpha level from p = 0.05 to p = 0.005, is deleterious for the finding of new discoveries and the progress of science. Given that blanket and variable alpha levels both are problematic, it is sensible to dispense with significance testing altogether. There are alternatives that address study design and sample size much more directly than significance testing does; but none of the statistical tools should be taken as the new magic method giving clear-cut mechanical answers. Inference should not be based on single studies at all, but on cumulative evidence from multiple independent studies. When evaluating the strength of the evidence, we should consider, for example, auxiliary assumptions, the strength of the experimental design, and implications for applications. To boil all this down to a binary decision based on a p-value threshold of 0.05, 0.01, 0.005, or anything else, is not acceptable. KW - statistical significance KW - null hypothesis testing KW - p-value KW - significance testing KW - decision making Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-189973 SN - 1664-1078 VL - 9 IS - 699 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Gromer, Daniel A1 - Madeira, Octávia A1 - Gast, Philipp A1 - Nehfischer, Markus A1 - Jost, Michael A1 - Müller, Mathias A1 - Mühlberger, Andreas A1 - Pauli, Paul T1 - Height Simulation in a Virtual Reality CAVE System: Validity of Fear Responses and Effects of an Immersion Manipulation JF - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience N2 - Acrophobia is characterized by intense fear in height situations. Virtual reality (VR) can be used to trigger such phobic fear, and VR exposure therapy (VRET) has proven effective for treatment of phobias, although it remains important to further elucidate factors that modulate and mediate the fear responses triggered in VR. The present study assessed verbal and behavioral fear responses triggered by a height simulation in a 5-sided cave automatic virtual environment (CAVE) with visual and acoustic simulation and further investigated how fear responses are modulated by immersion, i.e., an additional wind simulation, and presence, i.e., the feeling to be present in the VE. Results revealed a high validity for the CAVE and VE in provoking height related self-reported fear and avoidance behavior in accordance with a trait measure of acrophobic fear. Increasing immersion significantly increased fear responses in high height anxious (HHA) participants, but did not affect presence. Nevertheless, presence was found to be an important predictor of fear responses. We conclude that a CAVE system can be used to elicit valid fear responses, which might be further enhanced by immersion manipulations independent from presence. These results may help to improve VRET efficacy and its transfer to real situations. KW - anxiety KW - fear behavior KW - virtual reality KW - presence KW - immersion KW - acrophobia Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-196113 SN - 1662-5161 VL - 12 IS - 372 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Rodrigues, Johannes A1 - Nagowski, Natalie A1 - Mussel, Patrick A1 - Hewig, Johannes T1 - Altruistic punishment is connected to trait anger, not trait altruism, if compensation is available JF - Heliyon N2 - Altruistic punishment and altruistic compensation are important concepts that are used to investigate altruism. However, altruistic punishment has been found to be correlated with anger. We were interested whether altruistic punishment and altruistic compensation are both driven by trait altruism and trait anger or whether the influence of those two traits is more specific to one of the behavioral options. We found that if the participants were able to apply altruistic compensation and altruistic punishment together in one paradigm, trait anger only predicts altruistic punishment and trait altruism only predicts altruistic compensation. Interestingly, these relations are disguised in classical altruistic punishment and altruistic compensation paradigms where participants can either only punish or compensate. Hence altruistic punishment and altruistic compensation paradigms should be merged together if one is interested in trait altruism without the confounding influence of trait anger. KW - psychology KW - altruism KW - altruistic punishment KW - altruistic compensation KW - anger Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-177669 VL - 4 IS - 11 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Roth, Jenny A1 - Steffens, Melanie C. A1 - Vignoles, Vivian L. T1 - Group membership, group change, and intergroup attitudes: a recategorization model based on cognitive consistency principles JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - The present article introduces a model based on cognitive consistency principles to predict how new identities become integrated into the self-concept, with consequences for intergroup attitudes. The model specifies four concepts (self-concept, stereotypes, identification, and group compatibility) as associative connections. The model builds on two cognitive principles, balance-congruity and imbalance-dissonance, to predict identification with social groups that people currently belong to, belonged to in the past, or newly belong to. More precisely, the model suggests that the relative strength of self-group associations (i.e., identification) depends in part on the (in)compatibility of the different social groups. Combining insights into cognitive representation of knowledge, intergroup bias, and explicit/implicit attitude change, we further derive predictions for intergroup attitudes. We suggest that intergroup attitudes alter depending on the relative associative strength between the social groups and the self, which in turn is determined by the (in)compatibility between social groups. This model unifies existing models on the integration of social identities into the self-concept by suggesting that basic cognitive mechanisms play an important role in facilitating or hindering identity integration and thus contribute to reducing or increasing intergroup bias. KW - cognitive balance KW - cognitive dissonance KW - group change KW - identity integration KW - intergroup bias KW - social identification KW - recategorization KW - prejudice Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-175569 VL - 9 IS - 479 ER - TY - THES A1 - Botrel, Loic T1 - Brain-computer interfaces (BCIs) based on sensorimotor rhythms - Evaluating practical interventions to improve their performance and reduce BCI inefficiency T1 - Gehirn-Computer Schnittstellen (BCIs) basierend auf sensomotorischen Rhythmen - Evaluation praktischer Interventionen zur Verbesserung ihrer Leistung und Reduktion von BCI Ineffizienz N2 - Brain computer interfaces based on sensorimotor rhythms modulation (SMR-BCIs) allow people to emit commands to an interface by imagining right hand, left hand or feet movements. The neurophysiological activation associated with those specific mental imageries can be measured by electroencephalography and detected by machine learning algorithms. Improvements for SMR-BCI accuracy in the last 30 years seem to have reached a limit. The currrent main issue with SMR-BCIs is that between 15% to 30% cannot use the BCI, called the "BCI inefficiency" issue. Alternatively to hardware and software improvements, investigating the individual characteristics of the BCI users has became an interesting approach to overcome BCI inefficiency. In this dissertation, I reviewed existing literature concerning the individual sources of variation in SMR-BCI accuracy and identified generic individual characteristics. In the empirical investigation, attention and motor dexterity predictors for SMR-BCI performance were implemented into a trainings that would manipulate those predictors and lead to higher SMR-BCI accuracy. Those predictors were identified by Hammer et al. (2012) as the ability to concentrate (associated with relaxation levels) and "mean error duration" in a two-hand visuo-motor coordination task (VMC). Prior to a SMR-BCI session, a total of n=154 participants in two locations took part of 23 min sessions of either Jacobson’s Progressive Muscle Relaxation session (PMR), a VMC session, or a control group (CG). No effect of PMR or VMC manipulation was found, but the manipulation checks did not consistently confirm whether PMR had an effect of relaxation levels and VMC on "mean error duration". In this first study, correlations between relaxation levels or "mean error duration" and accuracy were found but not in both locations. A second study, involving n=39 participants intensified the training in four sessions on four consecutive days or either PMR, VMC or CG. The effect or manipulation was assessed for in terms of a causal relationship by using a PRE-POST study design. The manipulation checks of this second study validated the positive effect of training on both relaxation and "mean error duration". But the manipulation did not yield a specific effect on BCI accuracy. The predictors were not found again, displaying the instability of relaxation levels and "mean error duration" in being associated with BCI performance. An effect of time on BCI accuracy was found, and a correlation between State Mindfulness Scale and accuracy were reported. Results indicated that a short training of PMR or VMC were insufficient in increasing SMR-BCI accuracy. This study contrasted with studies succeeding in increasing SMR-BCI accuracy Tan et al. (2009, 2014), by the shortness of its training and the relaxation training that did not include mindfulness. It also contrasted by its manipulation checks and its comprehensive experimental approach that attempted to replicate existing predictors or correlates for SMR-BCI accuracy. The prediction of BCI accuracy by individual characteristics is receiving increased attention, but requires replication studies and a comprehensive approach, to contribute to the growing base of evidence of predictors for SMR-BCI accuracy. While short PMR and VMC trainings could not yield an effect on BCI performance, mindfulness meditation training might be beneficial for SMR-BCI accuracy. Moreover, it could be implemented for people in the locked-in-syndrome, allowing to reach the end-users that are the most in need for improvements in BCI performance. N2 - Les interfaces cerveau-ordinateur (angl. brain-computer interfaces, BCIs) basées sur les rythmes sensorimoteurs (angl. sensorimotor rhythms, SMR) permettent d’émettre des commandes par l’imagination de mouvements des mains ou des jambes. Dans le cas des BCIs non-invasifs, les manifestations neurophysiologiques liées á l’imagination motrice peuvent être mesurées par électroencephalographie (EEG) á la surface du cuir chevelu, puis détectées á l’aide d’algorithmes d’apprentissage. Après 30 années de progrès dans l’implémentation des BCI basées sur les SMR, il devient de plus en plus difficile d’obtenir un gain significatif de performance, alors qu’il est estimé qu’entre 15% et 30 % des utilisateurs ne peuvent pas utiliser une BCI basée sur les SMR. On parle d’inefficacité de la BCI (angl. BCI inefficiency). Une alternative aux avancées matérielles et logicielles réside dans l’investigation de caractéristiques propres à l’utilisateur. Dans ce travail de thèse, j’ai d’abord procédé à une revue de littérature sur les sources individuelles de variation de la performance SMR-BCIs, sous la forme de caractéristiques psychologiques, neurologiques et neuroanatomiques propres à l’utilisateur. Pour l’étude empirique, je me suis basé sur deux prédicteurs – l’attention et la dextérite motrice – que j’ai expérimentalement manipulés par des protocoles d’intervention. Ces deux prédicteurs ont été identifiés par Hammer et al. (2012) en tant que capacité à se concentrer (ability to concentrate) et durée moyenne d’erreur dans une tâche de coordination visuo-motrice (mean error duration in a visuomotor coordination task, VMC). La première étude comprend N=154 participants recrutés dans deux villes allemandes (Würzburg et Berlin). Avant de procéder à une session de BCI basée sur les SMR, les participants ont été aléatoirement répartis en trois groupes d’intervention d’une durée de 23 minutes. Le groupe PMR a pris part à une session de relaxation musculaire progressive de Jacobson, censée relaxer le participant ; le groupe VMC a pris part a une session de coordination visuo-motrice des deux mains, censé augmenter la dextérité motrice ; le groupe controle CG ayant eu pour tâche de lire un texte. Les résultats, analysés indépendemment pour chaque lieu de mesure, indiquent que l’entraînement PMR ou VMC n’ont pas provoqué d’amélioration significative de la performance BCI. L’effet des interventions sur leurs variables témoins respectives (PMR sur le niveau subjectif de relaxation ; VMC sur la durée moyenne d’erreur) sont inéquivoques. Il n’est donc pas possible d’interpréter l’absence d’effet d’entraînement sur la performance BCI. Les corrélations entre les variables témoins et la performance BCI répliquent les deux prédicteurs à l’origine de l’étude, mais ces résultats sont restreints à l’un des deux lieux de mesure. La seconde étude a été menée sur N=39 participants pour lesquels la durée d’entraînement (soit PMR, VMC ou CG) a été prolongée sur quatre sessions étalées sur quatre jours successifs. Cette seconde étude a été conçue selon un modèle pré-test post-test permettant de réduire la sensibilité aux variations inter-individuelles de la performance, ainsi que de tester la présence d’une relation causale entre entraînement et performance BCI. Les variables témoins – relaxation et durée d’erreur VMC– ont evolué de maniere positive validant les entraînements. Cependant, les entraînements PMR et VMC n’ont eu aucun effet positif sur la performance BCI basée sur les SMR. Les prédicteurs n’ont donc pas de nouveau été répliqués, démontrant l’instabilité des niveaux de relaxation et la performance VMC dans leur association avec la performance BCI. L’effet de temps sur la performance BCI, constaté dans de nombreuses études a été répliqué. De manière plus inattendue, une correlation entre l’échelle d’attention consciente (state mindfulness scale, SMS) et la performance BCI a été révélée. Globalement, Les résultats de ces deux études empiriques indiquent que de courts entraînements PMR ou VMC ont été insuffisants pour améliorer la performance BCI. Ces études contrastent donc avec les précédentes études qui au contraîre ont montré un effect positif d’un entraînement en relaxation Tan et al. (2009, 2014), notamment marqués par leur durée s’étalant sur plusieurs mois ainsi que leur forme de relaxations basées sur la méditation de pleine conscience (angl. Mindfulness). Mes deux études se démarquent cependant par la présence de tests de manipulation, l’approche expérimentale basée sur l’implémentation du potentiel des prédicteurs et corrélats de la performance BCI. La prédiction de performance SMR-BCI par des caractérisiques individuelles reçevant une attention croissante ces dernières années, il est nécessaire pour contribuer efficacement au domaine des sources de variation des BCI, d’opter pour une approche expériementale englobant les résultats existants, notamment par l’effort de réplication, et de comparaison d’études. En conclusion, Alors que de courts entraînements PMR et VMC n’ont pas eu d’effets sur la performance BCI basée sur les SMR, la piste de l’entraînement de méditation pleine conscience présente un potentiel qu’il est nécessaire de confirmer. De plus, il pourraît être mis en place pour des patients paralysés moteur (angl. locked-in syndrome, LIS), permettant de fait d’atteîndre la population pouvant le plus profiter des améliorations de la performance BCI. N2 - Gehirn-Computer Schnittstellen (engl. brain-computer interfaces, BCIs), basierend auf der Modulation sensomotorischer Rhythmen (SMR), erlauben Menschen, Befehle an eine Schnittstelle zu übermitteln, beispielsweise durch die Vorstellung von Bewegungen der Hände oder der Füße. Die neurophysiologische Aktivität, die mit den Bewegungsvorstellungen assoziiert ist, kann mittels Elektroenzephalographie gemessen und durch Algorithmen aus dem Bereich des maschinellen Lernens detektiert werden. Die Fortschritte in Bezug auf SMR-BCIs, die es in den letzten 30 Jahren gab, scheinen an eine Grenze zu stoßen. Das Hauptproblem liegt darin, dass 15 bis 30% der Nutzer keine Kontrolle über SMR-BCIs erlangen. Dieses Phänomen wird als „BCI Ineffizienz“ bezeichnet. Neben Verbesserungen der Hard- und Software ist die Untersuchung individueller Charakteristika der BCI Nutzer ein vielversprechender Ansatz, um die BCI Ineffizienz zu überwinden. Im Rahmen dieser Dissertation habe ich zunächst durch eine Literaturstudie zu den Ursachen der Variation der SMR-BCI Genauigkeiten individuelle Charakteristika identifiziert. In der experimentellen Untersuchung wurden Aufmerksamkeit und Feinmotorik als Prädiktoren für die Leistung mit einem SMR-BCI in ein Trainingsparadigma aufgenommen, das zum Ziel hatte, die SMR-BCI Genauigkeiten zu verbessern. Diese Prädiktoren wurden von Hammer et al. (2012) als die Konzentrationsfähigkeit (assoziiert mit Entspannungsniveau) und „mittlere Fehlerdauer“ in einer beidhändigen visuomotorischen Koordinationsaufgabe (engl. two-hand visuo-motor coordination task, VMC) identifiziert. In der ersten Studie der vorliegenden Dissertation nahmen insgesamt n=154 Studienteilnehmer an zwei verschiedenen Standorten teil. Im Vorfeld einer SMR-BCI Sitzung nahmen diese entweder an einer 23-minütigen Sitzung mit Progressiver Muskelrelaxation nach Jacobson (PMR), einer Sitzung mit VMC oder einer Kontrollgruppe (KG) teil. Es zeigten sich keine Effekte auf die Genauigkeiten des SMRBCI als Folge der Versuchsbedingung (VMC, PMR oder KG). Jedoch konnte auch durch Manipulationschecks nicht konsistent bestätigt werden, dass PMR eine Auswirkung auf das Entspannungsniveau und VMC auf die „mittlere Fehlerdauer“ hatte. In dieser ersten Studie konnten Korrelationen zwischen dem Entspannungsniveau oder „mittlerer Fehlerdauer“ und der Genauigkeit mit dem SMR-BCI aufgedeckt werden, jedoch nicht an beiden Standorten. In der zweiten Studie dieser Dissertation mit n=39 Teilnehmern wurde das Training durch die Steigerung auf vier Sitzungen intensiviert, die an vier aufeinanderfolgenden Tagen entweder mit PMR, VMC oder KG durchgeführt wurden. Der Effekt dieser Manipulation auf SMRBCI Genauigkeiten wurde mittels eines Pretest-Posttest-Studiendesigns untersucht. Die Manipulationschecks validierten den positiven Effekt des Trainings sowohl für Entspannung als auch die „mittlere Fehlerdauer“. Es gab jedoch keine spezifische Wirkung des Trainings auf die BCI Genauigkeiten. Entspannungsniveau und „mittlere Fehlerdauer“ konnten nicht als zuverlässige Prädiktoren für SMR-BCI Leistung bestätigt werden. Es gab einen Effekt der Zeit auf die BCI Genauigkeit und eine Korrelation zwischen der State Mindfulness Scale und der Genauigkeit. Die Ergebnisse deuten darauf hin, dass ein kurzes PMR oder VMC Training nicht ausreichten, um SMR-BCI Genauigkeiten zu steigern. Diese Studie steht im Widerspruch zu Studien von Tan et al. (2009, 2014), die erfolgreich die SMR-BCI Genauigkeit steigern konnten, unterscheidet sich von diesen jedoch auch durch die kürzere Trainingsdauer und dem Fehlen von Achtsamkeitskomponenten beim Entspannungstraining. Weitere Unterschiede liegen in dem verwendeten Manipulationscheck und dem umfassenden experimentellen Ansatz der aktuellen Studie mit dem Ziel, zuvor ermittelte Prädiktoren oder Korrelate von SMR-BCI Genauigkeit zu replizieren. Die Vorhersage von BCI Genauigkeit durch individuelle Charakteristika erhält steigende wissenschaftliche Aufmerksamkeit, bedarf aber Replikationsstudien und eines umfassenden Ansatzes, um die Beweislage hinsichtlich Prädiktoren für SMR-BCI Genauigkeit zu verbessern. Während für kurze PMR und VMC Trainings kein Effekt auf die SMR-BCI Genauigkeit aufgedeckt werden konnte, könnte sich achtsamkeitsbasiertes Meditationstraining als vorteilhaft für die Leistung mit einem SMR-BCI erweisen. Darüber hinaus könnte es auch für Personen mit Locked-In-Syndrom implementiert werden, um so diejenigen Endnutzer zu erreichen, die am meisten von Verbesserungen der BCI Leistung profitieren würden. KW - Gehirn-Computer-Schnittstelle KW - Locked-in-Syndrom KW - Intervention KW - Sensorimotor Rhythms KW - visuomotor coordination KW - predictor analysis KW - Psychologie KW - Progressive Relaxation Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-168110 ER - TY - THES A1 - Alvarez Loeblich, Paul Sebastian T1 - Not Here, Not Now!
 – Situational Appropriateness, Negative Affect and the Experience of (Remote) Embarrassment. A Process Model. T1 - Nicht hier, nicht jetzt! – Situationelle Angemessenheit, negativer Affekt und die Erfahrung von (Fern-) Peinlichkeit. Ein Prozessmodell. N2 - Fremdschämen or Fremdscham, a negative emotion which arises while observing someone behave inappropriately, comes to fame after the turn of the millennium in german speaking countries. There, they name it literally „other‘s shame“ and it becomes obvious that this emotion happens most commonly while watching TV: reality shows, talent shows and bad comedies. The word even makes it to the dictionaries starting 2009, as its use increases unstoppably in everyday language, starting to get used in more and more situations, seemingly as a synonym of embarrassing or shameful. Still, a look in the emotional research on the subject returns exactly zero results as of 2011, leaving open the question as of what this emotion might be, and what it is not. The present wort aims at explaining not only the phenomenon of Fremdschämen, but also the Emotion behind it - Embarrassment -, at a process level. N2 - Bisher blieb die wahrscheinlich universelle Emotion Peinlichkeit unzureichend erforscht. Grundsätzlich beschränkt sich die Forschung zum Thema auf heuristische Erklärungsansätze und die Empirie auf lustige Methoden, diese Emotion zu erzeugen. Die Prozesse hinter der Entstehung der Emotion sind bis dato weitestgehend vernachlässigt worden und haben kaum Berücksichtigung in der Forschung gefunden. Zudem gerieten in den letzten Jahren die meisten existierenden Erklärungsansätze unter Druck, als die ersten Fragen zum Thema Fremdscham bzw. Fremdschämen in einen wissenschaftlichen Rahmen gestellt wurden. Denn die offensichtliche phänomenologische Ähnlichkeit von Peinlichkeit und Fremdschämen kann von keinem der bisherigen Erklärungsansätze widerspruchsfrei durchleuchtet werden. Dies liegt unter anderen Dingen auch darin begründet, dass selbst die Emotion Peinlichkeit kaum auf Prozessebene erforscht wurde. In dieser Arbeit wird daher ein neues Verständnis der Emotion Peinlichkeit angestrebt. Eines, bei dem Fremdscham Berücksichtigung findet und das dieses Phänomen ohne Denkakrobatik befriedigend erklären kann. Ein Prozessmodell für die Entstehung von Peinlichkeit, das ebenso Fremdscham miteinschließt, wird vorgestellt. KW - Sozialpsychologie KW - Peinlichkeit KW - Zwei-Prozess-Modell KW - Affekt KW - Fremdschämen KW - Fremdscham Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-161354 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Grossheinrich, Nicola A1 - Firk, Christine A1 - Schulte-Rüther, Martin A1 - von Leupoldt, Andreas A1 - Konrad, Kerstin A1 - Huestegge, Lynn T1 - Looking while unhappy: a mood-congruent attention bias toward sad adult faces in children JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - A negative mood-congruent attention bias has been consistently observed, for example, in clinical studies on major depression. This bias is assumed to be dysfunctional in that it supports maintaining a sad mood, whereas a potentially adaptive role has largely been neglected. Previous experiments involving sad mood induction techniques found a negative mood-congruent attention bias specifically for young individuals, explained by an adaptive need for information transfer in the service of mood regulation. In the present study we investigated the attentional bias in typically developing children (aged 6–12 years) when happy and sad moods were induced. Crucially, we manipulated the age (adult vs. child) of the displayed pairs of facial expressions depicting sadness, anger, fear and happiness. The results indicate that sad children indeed exhibited a mood specific attention bias toward sad facial expressions. Additionally, this bias was more pronounced for adult faces. Results are discussed in the context of an information gain which should be stronger when looking at adult faces due to their more expansive life experience. These findings bear implications for both research methods and future interventions. KW - eye tracking KW - emotion regulation KW - mood induction KW - attention bias KW - major depression KW - adaptive role Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-177688 VL - 9 IS - 2577 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Ventura-Bort, Carlos A1 - Wirkner, Janine A1 - Genheimer, Hannah A1 - Wendt, Julia A1 - Hamm, Alfons O. A1 - Weymar, Mathias T1 - Effects of Transcutaneous Vagus Nerve Stimulation (tVNS) on the P300 and Alpha-Amylase Level: A Pilot Study JF - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience N2 - Recent research suggests that the P3b may be closely related to the activation of the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine (LC-NE) system. To further study the potential association, we applied a novel technique, the non-invasive transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation (tVNS), which is speculated to increase noradrenaline levels. Using a within-subject cross-over design, 20 healthy participants received continuous tVNS and sham stimulation on two consecutive days (stimulation counterbalanced across participants) while performing a visual oddball task. During stimulation, oval non-targets (standard), normal-head (easy) and rotated-head (difficult) targets, as well as novel stimuli (scenes) were presented. As an indirect marker of noradrenergic activation we also collected salivary alpha-amylase (sAA) before and after stimulation. Results showed larger P3b amplitudes for target, relative to standard stimuli, irrespective of stimulation condition. Exploratory post hoc analyses, however, revealed that, in comparison to standard stimuli, easy (but not difficult) targets produced larger P3b (but not P3a) amplitudes during active tVNS, compared to sham stimulation. For sAA levels, although main analyses did not show differential effects of stimulation, direct testing revealed that tVNS (but not sham stimulation) increased sAA levels after stimulation. Additionally, larger differences between tVNS and sham stimulation in P3b magnitudes for easy targets were associated with larger increase in sAA levels after tVNS, but not after sham stimulation. Despite preliminary evidence for a modulatory influence of tVNS on the P3b, which may be partly mediated by activation of the noradrenergic system, additional research in this field is clearly warranted. Future studies need to clarify whether tVNS also facilitates other processes, such as learning and memory, and whether tVNS can be used as therapeutic tool. KW - EEG KW - P300 KW - tVNS KW - norepinephrine KW - locus coeruleus KW - salivary alpha-amylase Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-196129 SN - 1662-5161 VL - 12 IS - 202 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Hommers, L. G. A1 - Richter, J. A1 - Yang, Y. A1 - Raab, A. A1 - Baumann, C. A1 - Lang, K. A1 - Schiele, M. A. A1 - Weber, H. A1 - Wittmann, A. A1 - Wolf, C. A1 - Alpers, G. W. A1 - Arolt, V. A1 - Domschke, K. A1 - Fehm, L. A1 - Fydrich, T. A1 - Gerlach, A. A1 - Gloster, A. T. A1 - Hamm, A. O. A1 - Helbig-Lang, S. A1 - Kircher, T. A1 - Lang, T. A1 - Pané-Farré, C. A. A1 - Pauli, P. A1 - Pfleiderer, B. A1 - Reif, A. A1 - Romanos, M. A1 - Straube, B. A1 - Ströhle, A. A1 - Wittchen, H.-U. A1 - Frantz, S. A1 - Ertl, G. A1 - Lohse, M. J. A1 - Lueken, U. A1 - Deckert, J. T1 - A functional genetic variation of SLC6A2 repressor hsa-miR-579-3p upregulates sympathetic noradrenergic processes of fear and anxiety JF - Translational Psychiatry N2 - Increased sympathetic noradrenergic signaling is crucially involved in fear and anxiety as defensive states. MicroRNAs regulate dynamic gene expression during synaptic plasticity and genetic variation of microRNAs modulating noradrenaline transporter gene (SLC6A2) expression may thus lead to altered central and peripheral processing of fear and anxiety. In silico prediction of microRNA regulation of SLC6A2 was confirmed by luciferase reporter assays and identified hsa-miR-579-3p as a regulating microRNA. The minor (T)-allele of rs2910931 (MAFcases = 0.431, MAFcontrols = 0.368) upstream of MIR579 was associated with panic disorder in patients (pallelic = 0.004, ncases = 506, ncontrols = 506) and with higher trait anxiety in healthy individuals (pASI = 0.029, pACQ = 0.047, n = 3112). Compared to the major (A)-allele, increased promoter activity was observed in luciferase reporter assays in vitro suggesting more effective MIR579 expression and SLC6A2 repression in vivo (p = 0.041). Healthy individuals carrying at least one (T)-allele showed a brain activation pattern suggesting increased defensive responding and sympathetic noradrenergic activation in midbrain and limbic areas during the extinction of conditioned fear. Panic disorder patients carrying two (T)-alleles showed elevated heart rates in an anxiety-provoking behavioral avoidance test (F(2, 270) = 5.47, p = 0.005). Fine-tuning of noradrenaline homeostasis by a MIR579 genetic variation modulated central and peripheral sympathetic noradrenergic activation during fear processing and anxiety. This study opens new perspectives on the role of microRNAs in the etiopathogenesis of anxiety disorders, particularly their cardiovascular symptoms and comorbidities. KW - clinical genetics KW - psychiatric disorders Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-322497 VL - 8 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Holtfrerich, Sarah K. C. A1 - Pfister, Roland A1 - El Gammal, Alexander T. A1 - Bellon, Eugen A1 - Diekhof, Esther K. T1 - Endogenous testosterone and exogenous oxytocin influence the response to baby schema in the female brain JF - Scientific Reports N2 - Nurturing behavior may be critically influenced by the interplay of different hormones. The neuropeptide oxytocin is known to promote maternal behavior and its reduction has been associated with postpartum depression risk and child neglect. Contrariwise, the observed decrease in testosterone level during early parenthood may benefit caretaking behavior, whereas increased testosterone may reduce attention to infants. Here we used functional magnetic resonance imaging to investigate the interactive influence of testosterone and oxytocin on selective attention to and neural processing of the baby schema (BS). 57 nulliparous women performed a target detection task with human faces with varying degree of BS following double-blinded placebo-controlled oxytocin administration in a between-subjects design. Our results support the idea that oxytocin enhances attention to the BS. Oxytocin had a positive effect on activation of the inferior frontal junction during identification of infant targets with a high degree of BS that were presented among adult distractors. Further, activation of the putamen was positively correlated with selective attention to the BS, but only in women with high endogenous testosterone who received oxytocin. These findings provide initial evidence for the neural mechanism by which oxytocin may counteract the negative effects of testosterone in the modulation of nurturing behavior. KW - attention KW - reward Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-322285 VL - 8 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Hammer, Eva M. A1 - Halder, Sebastian A1 - Kleih, Sonja C. A1 - Kübler, Andrea T1 - Psychological Predictors of Visual and Auditory P300 Brain-Computer Interface Performance JF - Frontiers in Neuroscience N2 - Brain-Computer Interfaces (BCIs) provide communication channels independent from muscular control. In the current study we used two versions of the P300-BCI: one based on visual the other on auditory stimulation. Up to now, data on the impact of psychological variables on P300-BCI control are scarce. Hence, our goal was to identify new predictors with a comprehensive psychological test-battery. A total of N = 40 healthy BCI novices took part in a visual and an auditory BCI session. Psychological variables were measured with an electronic test-battery including clinical, personality, and performance tests. The personality factor “emotional stability” was negatively correlated (Spearman's rho = −0.416; p < 0.01) and an output variable of the non-verbal learning test (NVLT), which can be interpreted as ability to learn, correlated positively (Spearman's rho = 0.412; p < 0.01) with visual P300-BCI performance. In a linear regression analysis both independent variables explained 24% of the variance. “Emotional stability” was also negatively related to auditory P300-BCI performance (Spearman's rho = −0.377; p < 0.05), but failed significance in the regression analysis. Psychological parameters seem to play a moderate role in visual P300-BCI performance. “Emotional stability” was identified as a new predictor, indicating that BCI users who characterize themselves as calm and rational showed worse BCI performance. The positive relation of the ability to learn and BCI performance corroborates the notion that also for P300 based BCIs learning may constitute an important factor. Further studies are needed to consolidate or reject the presented predictors. KW - predictors KW - visual P300-BCI KW - auditory P300-BCI KW - NVLT KW - emotional stability Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-369207 VL - 12 ER - TY - THES A1 - Flohr, Elena Leonie Ruth T1 - The Scents of Interpersonality - On the Influence of Smells on the Evaluation and Processing of Social Stimuli T1 - Die Düfte der Zwischenmenschlichkeit - Über den Einfluss von Gerüchen auf die Bewertung und Verarbeitung von sozialen Reizen N2 - In daily life, olfactory stimuli are potential generators of affective states, but also have a strong influence on social interaction. Pleasant odors have been shown to increase perceived attractiveness and pro-social behavior, whereas unpleasant body odors are often associated with negative personality traits. Since both pleasant odors and positive affective state facilitate pro-social behavior, it is conceivable that the influence of the odors on social interaction is mediated by the induced affective state elicited by the odor itself. The present thesis aims at exploring the impact of hedonic, i.e., pleasant or unpleasant, odors on the processing and evaluation of social stimuli as assessed by verbal, physiological, and behavioral indices. First, I investigate the effects of initially neutral odors which gained threatening value through an aversive conditioning procedure on social stimuli (Study 1). Second, I study the influence of naturally hedonic odors on social interaction. Third, this thesis aims at disentangling differences in the effects of an odor attributed to either a social interaction partner or the environment where the social encounter takes place (Study 2, 3, and 4). In the first study, a context conditioning procedure was applied, during which one out of two long-lasting neutral odors was paired with an unpredictable aversive unconditioned stimulus (US, i.e., white noise). This odor (CTX+) thereby gained threatening value, while another odor (CTX-) remained unpaired and therefore signaled safety. During a test session, facial stimuli were presented within both conditioned olfactory contexts. Results indicate that autonomic arousal was increased to faces when presented in the threatening odor context. Additionally, participants rated facial stimuli as more aversive when presented in the threatening odor as compared to the safety odor, indicating that faces acquire hedonic value from the odor they were presented in. Strikingly, angry facial expressions received additional processing resources when presented within a threatening olfactory context, as reflected on verbal reports and electrodermal activity (EDA). This latter finding suggests that threat-related stimuli, here angry faces, are preferentially processed within an olfactory context where a threat might happen. Considering that the hedonic value of an odor may be quite subjective, I conducted a pilot study in order to identify odors with pleasant vs. unpleasant properties for most participants. Seven odors (four pleasant and three unpleasant) were rated with respect to their valence (pleasant vs. unpleasant), arousal (arousing vs. calm), and intensity. Additionally, EDA was measured. Two pleasant (Citral and Eucalyptol) and two unpleasant (“Animalis” and Isobutyraldehyde) odors were chosen from the original seven. The unpleasant odors were rated as more negative, arousing, and intense than the positive ones, but no differences were found regarding EDA. These four odors were subsequently used in a virtual reality (VR) paradigm with two odor attribution groups. Participants of the social attribution group (n = 59) were always passively guided into the same room (an office) towards one out of two virtual agents who were either paired with the pleasant or the unpleasant odor. Participants of the contextual attribution group (n = 58) were guided into one out of two rooms which were either paired with the pleasant or the unpleasant odor and where they always met the same agent. For both groups, the agents smiled, frowned or remained with a neutral facial expression. This design allowed evaluating the influence of odor valence as a within-subjects factor and the influence of odor attribution as a between-subjects factor. Unpleasant odors facilitated the processing of social cues as reflected by increased verbal and physiological arousal as well as reduced active approach behavior. Specific influence of odor valence on emotional facial expressions was found for ratings, EDA, and facial mimicry, with the unpleasant odor causing a levelling effect on the differences between facial expressions. The social attribution group exhibited larger differences between odors than the contextual group with respect to some variables (i.e., ratings and EDA), but not to others (i.e., electrocortical potentials – ERPs – and approach behavior). In sum, unpleasant in comparison to pleasant odors diminished emotional responses during social interaction, while an additional enhancing effect of the social attribution was observed on some variables. Interestingly, the awareness that an interaction partner would smell (pleasantly or unpleasantly) boosted the emotional reactivity towards them. In Study 3, I adapted the VR paradigm to a within-subjects design, meaning that the different attribution conditions were now manipulated block-wise. Instead of an approach task, participants had to move away from the virtual agent (withdrawal task). Results on the ratings were replicated from Study 2. Specifically, the difference between pleasant and unpleasant odors on valence, arousal, and sympathy ratings was larger in the social as compared to the contextual attribution condition. No effects of odor or attribution were found on EDA, whereas heart rate (HR) showed a stronger acceleration to pleasant odors while participants were passively guided towards the agent. Instead of an approach task, I focused on withdrawal behavior in this study. Interestingly, independently of the attribution condition, participants spent more time withdrawing from virtual agents, when an unpleasant odor was presented. In sum, I demonstrated that the attribution of the odors to the social agent itself had an enhancing effect on their influence on social interaction. In the fourth and last study, I applied a similar within-subjects protocol as in Study 3 with an additional Ultimatum Game task as a measure of social interaction. Overall findings replicated the results of Study 3 with respect to HR and EDA. Strikingly, participants offered less money to virtual agents in the bad smelling room than in the good smelling room. In contrast to Study 3, no effects of odor attribution were found in Study 4. In sum, again I demonstrated that unpleasant odor may lessen social interaction not only when the interaction partner smells badly, but also in more complex interaction situations. In conclusion, I demonstrated that hedonic odors in general influence social interaction. Thus, pleasant odors seem to facilitate, while unpleasant odors seem to reduce interpersonal exchanges. Therefore, the present thesis extends the body of literature on the influence of odors on the processing of social stimuli. Although I found a direct influence of odors on social preferences as well as on the physiological and behavioral responses to social stimuli, I did not disentangle impact of odor per se from the impact of the affective state. Interestingly, odor attribution might play an additional role as mediator of social interactions such as odor effects in social interactions might be boosted when the smell is attributed to an individual. However, the results in this regard were less straightforward, and therefore further investigations are needed. Future research should also take into account gender or other inter-individual differences like social anxiety. N2 - Im täglichen Leben dienen Gerüche als starke Auslöser von emotionalen Zuständen, doch üben sie auch einen starken Einfluss auf soziale Interaktion aus. Angenehme Gerüche sollten die Attraktivität von Gesichtern und prosoziales Verhalten verstärken, während unangenehme Körpergerüche oft mit negativen Persönlichkeitseigenschaften assoziiert werden. Dieser Zusammenhang zeigt sich auch auf physiologischen und Verhaltensmaßen. Während angenehme Gerüche prosoziales Verhalten verstärken, kann derselbe Effekt auch durch einen positiven affektiven Zustand erreicht werden. Der Einfluss von Gerüchen auf soziale Interaktion könnte daher auch durch den affektiven Zustand der Versuchspersonen vermittelt werden. Die vorliegende Arbeit hatte zum Ziel, den Einfluss von hedonischen Gerüchen auf soziale Interaktion, wie er sich auf verschiedenen verbalen, physiologischen und Verhaltensvariablen abbilden lässt, darzustellen. Auf der einen Seite wurde der Einfluss von ursprünglich neutralen Gerüchen untersucht, die in einer Kontextkonditionierung bedrohliche Bedeutung erhielten (Studie 1). Auf der anderen Seite sollte der Einfluss eines Geruchs, der direkt von einem sozialen Interaktionspartner ausgeht, von dem eines eher kontextuellen Geruchs getrennt werden, der auf den Raum attribuiert wurde, in dem die soziale Interaktion stattfand (Studien 2, 3 und 4). In der ersten Studie wurde auf einen von zwei ursprünglich neutralen Gerüchen eine Kontextkonditionierungsprozedur angewandt. Dieser Geruch erhielt somit durch die Paarung mit einem aversiven unvorhersehbaren unkonditionierten Stimulus (US) bedrohliche Bedeutung, während der andere Geruch niemals mit einem unkonditionierten Stimulus gepaart wurde und dadurch Sicherheit signalisierte. In der Testphase wurden Gesichter entweder innerhalb des bedrohlichen Geruchs oder des Sicherheitsgeruchs präsentiert. Es konnte gezeigt werden, dass im Anschluss daran der bedrohliche Geruch die elektrokortikalen Potenziale (EKPs) auf Gesichter verstärkt, die in diesem Geruchskontext präsentiert werden. Zudem war das autonome Arousal während der Präsentation der Gesichsstimuli in diesem Kontext erhöht. Subjektive Ratings unterstützen zusätzlich die Annahme, dass die bedrohliche Bedeutung des Kontexts, in dem Gesichter präsentiert werden, auf diese übergeht. Zusätzlich zu diesem generellen Effekt konnte auf den subjektiven Ratings wie auch auf der elektrodermalen Aktivität (EDA) ein spezifischer Einfluss des olfaktorischen Kontexts auf die Verarbeitung von Gesichtern gezeigt werden. Ärgerliche Gesichter zogen dabei zusätzliche Verarbeitungsressourcen auf sich, wenn sie innerhalb eines bedrohlichen olfaktorischen Kontexts präsentiert wurden, wie sich auf der EDA und den verbalen Ratings zeigte. Zusammengefasst legen die letzteren Ergebnisse nahe, dass bedrohliche Reize (hier ärgerliche Gesichter) bevorzugt verarbeitet werden, wenn sie in einem ebenfalls bedrohlichen Kontext präsentiert werden. Um für die anschließenden Studien Gerüche zu identifizieren, die von den meisten Versuchspersonen als angenehm bzw. unangenehm bewertet werden, wurde vor der zweiten Studie eine Pilotstudie durchgeführt. Sieben Gerüche (vier angenehme und drei unangenehme) wurden bezüglich Valenz, Arousal und Intensität evaluiert. Zusätzlich wurde die EDA aufgezeichnet. Aus den ursprünglichen sieben Gerüchen wurden zwei angenehme (Citral und Eukalyptol) und zwei unangenehme („Animalis“ und Isobutanal) ausgewählt. Die unangenehmen Gerüche wurden als unangenehmer, aufregender und intensiver bewertet als die angenehmen, wohingegen in der EDA keine Unterschiede gefunden wurden. Studie 2 wandte die ausgewählten Gerüche in einem Experiment in virtueller Realität an. Um eine soziale und eine kontextuelle Attribution der Gerüche abzubilden, erhob ich zwei Attributions-gruppen. Versuchspersonen der sozialen Attributionsgruppe (n = 59) wurden passiv immer in denselben Raum geführt, in dem sie auf einen von zwei virtuellen Agenten trafen. Jeder dieser Agenten wurden mit entweder dem angenehmen oder dem unangenehmen Geruch gepaart. Probanden der kontextuellen Attributionsgruppe (n = 58) wurden jeweils passiv in einen von zwei Räumen geführt, der entweder mit dem angenehmen oder dem unangenehmen Geruch gepaart wurde. In diesen Räumen trafen sie immer auf denselben virtuellen Agenten. So war es möglich, den Einfluss der Geruchshedonik als Innersubjektfaktor und den Einfluss der Geruchsattribution als Zwischensubjektfaktor darzustellen. Der unangenehme Geruch erzeugte eine verstärkte Verarbeitung sozialer Reize, was sich in erhöhtem physiologischen Arousal, in subjektiven Ratings und vermindertem aktiven Annäherungsverhalten zeigte. Ein spezifischer Einfluss auf emotionale Gesichtsausdrücke war außerdem auf den subjektiven Ratings, EDA und der fazialen Mimikry zu beobachten. Hierbei zeigte sich ein abflachender Effekt auf den Unterschied zwischen den Gesichtsausdrücken, wenn der unangenehme Geruch präsentiert wurde. In der sozialen Attributionsgruppe fanden sich auf manchen Variablen stärkere Effekte als in der kontextuellen Attributionsgruppe (wie den Ratings und der EDA), aber auf anderen Variablen nicht (wie den EKPs und dem Annäherungsverhalten). Zusammenfassend konnte gezeigt werden, dass unangenehme Gerüche im Vergleich zu angenehmen emotionale Reaktionen auf soziale Interaktion vermindern. Ein zusätzlicher verstärkender Effekt durch die soziale Attribution der Gerüche war auf einigen Variablen zu beobachten. Interessanterweise scheint das Wissen darüber, dass ein Interaktionspartner riechen könnte, die emotionale Reaktion auf ihn zu verstärken. Für die dritte Studie passte ich das Paradigma für ein Innersubjektdesign an, wobei nun die beiden Attributionsbedingungen blockweise manipuliert wurden. Die Resultate der Ratings replizierten die aus Studie 2. Außerdem zeigten sich stärkere Effekte der Geruchsvalenz in der sozialen Attributionsbedingung auf allen Ratings. In der EDA wurden keine Effekte gefunden, aber in der Herzrate zeigte sich eine verstärkte Verarbeitung der angenehmen Gerüche während der passiven Annäherung an den Agenten. Statt des Annäherungsverhaltens wurde in dieser Studie das Rückzugsverhalten gemessen. Die Versuchspersonen verbrachten mehr Zeit damit, von einem Agenten zurückzuweichen, wenn ein unangenehmer Geruch präsentiert wurde. In Summe konnte ich zeigen, dass die Attribution der Gerüche auf den sozialen Agenten einen verstärkenden Effekt auf den Einfluss der Gerüche auf die soziale Interaktion hat. In der letzten Studie wurde dasselbe Protokoll wie in Studie 3 mit einer zusätzlichen Ultimatumspielaufgabe durchgeführt. Die Ergebnisse aus Studie 3 wurden bezüglich der Herzrate und der EDA repliziert. Außerdem boten die Versuchspersonen dem Agenten im Kontext eines unangenehmen Geruchs weniger Geld an als im Kontext eines angenehmen. In Studie 4 wurde kein Effekt für die Attribution des Geruchs gefunden. Zusammenfassend wurde gezeigt dass unangenehme Gerüche einen reduzierenden Effekt auf soziale Interaktion auch in komplexeren interaktiven Situationen ausüben. Zusammenfassend zeigte ich, dass Gerüche soziale Interaktion beeinflussen. Angenehme Gerüche scheinen soziale Interaktionen zu vereinfachen, während unangenehme Gerüche sie erschweren. Damit erweitert die vorliegende Arbeit bereits bestehende Forschung über den Einfluss von Gerüchen auf die Verarbeitung sozialer Stimuli. Obwohl ich einen direkten Einfluss von Gerüchen auf soziale Präferenzen sowie auf die physiologischen und behavioralen Reaktionen auf soziale Stimuli fand, konnte ich den Einfluss von Gerüchen per se nicht von dem Einfluss des affektiven Zustandes abgrenzen. Interessanterweise scheint die Attribution von Gerüchen einen zusätzlichen Faktor als Mediator von sozialen Interaktionen darzustellen, so dass der Effekt der Gerüche verstärkt wird, wenn er mit einem Individuum assoziiert ist. Nichtsdestotrotz waren die diesbezüglichen Effekte weniger klar und mehr Forschung auf diesem Gebiet könnte diese Unklarheit auflösen. Zukünftige Forschung sollte auch den Faktor Geschlecht nicht außer Acht lassen sowie andere inter-individuelle Unterschiede wie soziale Ängstlichkeit. KW - smell KW - social cognition KW - smells KW - social stimuli KW - social interaction Y1 - 2017 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-153352 ER - TY - THES A1 - Will, Sebastian T1 - Development of a presence model for driving simulators based on speed perception in a motorcycle riding simulator T1 - Entwicklung eines Präsenzmodells für Fahrsimulatoren basierend auf der Geschwindigkeitswahrnehmung in einem Motorradfahrsimulator N2 - Driving simulators are powerful research tools. Countless simulator studies have contributed to traffic safety over the last decades. Constant improvements in simulator technology call for a measureable scale to assess driving simulators with regard to their utility in human factors research. A promising psychological construct to do so is presence. It is commonly defined as the feeling of being located in a remote or virtual environment that seems to be real. Another aspect of presence describes the ability to act there successfully. The main aim of this thesis is to develop a presence model dedicated to the application in driving simulators. Established models have been combined and extended in order to gain a comprehensive model of presence that allows understanding its emergence and deriving recommendations on how to design or improve driving simulators. The five studies presented in this thesis investigate specific postulated model components and their interactions. All studies deal with motorcycling or a motorcycle riding simulator as exemplary field of application. The first study used a speed estimation task to investigate the contribution of different sensory cues to presence. While visualization plays a particularly important role, further improvements could be achieved by adding more consistent sensory stimuli to the virtual environment. Auditory, proprioceptive and vestibular cues have been subject to investigation. In the second study, the speed production method was applied. It confirmed the positive contribution of action to presence as predicted by psychocybernetic models. The third study dealt with the effect of training on presence. Hence, no positive effect was observed. The fourth study aimed at replicating previous findings on sensory fidelity and diversity in a more complex riding situation than only longitudinal vehicle control. The riders had to cross an unexpectedly appearing deep pit with the virtual motorcycle. The contribution of more consistent sensory stimulation on presence was successfully shown in this scenario, too. The final study was a real riding experiment that delivered reference values for the speed estimation capabilities of motorcycle riders. Besides higher variations in the simulator data, the general speed estimation performance was on a comparable level. Different measures, such as subjective ratings, behavioral responses, performance, and physiological reactions, have been applied as presence indicators. These studies’ findings deliver evidence for the meaningful application of the proposed presence model in driving simulator settings. The results suggest that presence can be interpreted as a quality measure for perception in virtual environments. In line with psychocybernetic models, taking action, which is seen as controlling perception, enhances this quality even further. Describing the psychological construct of presence in a theoretical framework that takes the diversity of perception and action in driving simulator settings into account closes a gap in traffic psychological research. N2 - Fahrsimulatoren sind leistungsfähige Forschungsinstrumente. Seit einigen Jahrzehnten konnte mit unzähligen Simulatorstudien zur Verkehrssicherheit beigetragen werden. Stetige Weiterentwicklungen der Simulatortechnologie machen einen Maßstab erforderlich, der es erlaubt, Fahrsimulatoren hinsichtlich ihrer Nützlichkeit für verkehrspsychologische Fragestellungen zu bewerten. Ein vielversprechendes in der Psychologie verwendetes Konstrukt ist Präsenz. Für gewöhnlich wird Präsenz als das Gefühl definiert, sich in einer entfernten bzw. virtuellen Umwelt zu befinden, die als real wahrgenommen wird. Ein weiterer Aspekt von Präsenz beschreibt die Fähigkeit in dieser Welt erfolgreich zu handeln. Das Ziel der vorliegenden Arbeit ist es, ein Präsenzmodell für die Anwendung im Fahrsimulatorbereich zu entwickeln. Dafür werden bereits etablierte Präsenzmodelle aufgegriffen, kombiniert und um bestimmte Komponenten erweitert. Dies zielt darauf ab ein umfassendes Präsenzmodell zu etablieren, welches einerseits einen Erklärungsansatz zur Entstehung von Präsenz liefert und andererseits erlaubt, Gestaltungsempfehlungen für Fahrsimulatoren abzuleiten. Die fünf Experimente dieser Arbeit untersuchen spezifische Modellkomponenten und deren Zusammenspiel. Alle Studien befassen sich mit dem Motorradfahren bzw. einem Motorradfahrsimulator als exemplarisches Anwendungsfeld. Die erste Studie verwendete ein Schätzverfahren für Geschwindigkeiten, um den Beitrag verschiedener Sinnesreize zu Präsenz zu untersuchen. Während der Visualisierung eine besondere Rolle zukommt, konnte die Präsenz durch die Hinzunahme weiterer sensorischer Stimuli in der virtuellen Welt noch gesteigert werden. Dabei wurden auditive, propriozeptive und vestibuläre Reize betrachtet. In der zweiten Studie wurde die Geschwindigkeitswahrnehmung mit Hilfe des Herstellungsverfahrens untersucht. Wie durch psychokybernetische Modelle vorhergesagt, hat sich die positive Auswirkung aktiven Handelns in der virtuellen Welt auf Präsenz bestätigt. Die dritte Studie befasste sich mit den Auswirkungen von Training auf Präsenz. Hier konnte jedoch kein positiver Zusammenhang festgestellt werden. Die vierte Studie zielte darauf ab, im Vorfeld gewonnene Erkenntnisse zur Vielfalt sensorischer Reize zu replizieren. Anstelle reiner Fahrzeuglängsregulation galt es, eine komplexere Fahrsituation zu bewältigen. Die Probanden mussten dabei mit ihrem virtuellen Motorrad einen unerwartet auftretenden tiefen Graben durchqueren. Der Beitrag vielfältiger sensorischer Stimulation auf Präsenz konnte auch in diesem Szenario erfolgreich gezeigt werden. Bei der letzten Studie handelte es sich um eine Realfahruntersuchung, die Referenzwerte zur Einordnung der Geschwindigkeitswahrnehmung im Motorradfahrsimulator lieferte. Außer höheren Schwankungen der Schätzwerte im Fahrsimulator bewegte sich die Güte der Schätzungen im Mittel auf einem vergleichbaren Niveau. Zur Messung von Präsenz wurden Befragungsdaten, Verhaltensmaße, Leistung und physiologische Reaktionen als Indikatoren herangezogen. Die Ergebnisse der Studien belegen die sinnvolle Anwendbarkeit des vorgeschlagenen Präsenzmodells in der Fahrsimulation. Darüber hinaus zeigt sich, dass Präsenz als ein Gütemaß für Wahrnehmung in virtuellen Welten interpretiert werden kann. Psychokybernetischen Modellen folgend kann diese Qualität durch Handeln, welches als Kontrolle der Wahrnehmung gesehen wird, noch weiter gesteigert werden. Durch die Integration des psychologischen Präsenzkonstrukts in ein Rahmenmodell, welches der Vielfalt von Wahrnehmung und Handlung in Fahrsimulatoren Rechnung trägt, konnte eine Lücke in der verkehrspsychologischen Forschung geschlossen werden. KW - Fahrsimulator KW - Geschwindigkeitswahrnehmung KW - presence KW - motorcycle simulator KW - driving simulator KW - speed perception KW - Präsenzerleben KW - Immersion KW - Telepräsenz Y1 - 2017 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-149748 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Pieczykolan, Aleks A1 - Huestegge, Lynn T1 - Cross-modal Action Complexity: Action- and Rule-related Memory Retrieval in Dual-response Control JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - Normally, we do not act within a single effector system only, but rather coordinate actions across several output modules (cross-modal action). Such cross-modal action demands can vary substantially with respect to their complexity in terms of the number of task-relevant response combinations and to-be-retrieved stimulus-response (S-R) mapping rules. In the present study, we study the impact of these two types of cross-modal action complexity on dual-response costs (i.e., performance differences between single- and dual-action demands). In Experiment 1, we combined a manual and an oculomotor task, each involving four response alternatives. Crucially, one (unconstrained) condition involved all 16 possible combinations of response alternatives, whereas a constrained condition involved only a subset of possible response combinations. The results revealed that preparing for a larger number of response combinations yielded a significant, but moderate increase in dual-response costs. In Experiment 2, we utilized one common lateralized auditory (e.g., left) stimulus to trigger incompatible response compounds (e.g., left saccade and right key press or vice versa). While one condition only involved one set of task-relevant S-R rules, another condition involved two sets of task-relevant rules (coded by stimulus type: noise/tone), while the number of task-relevant response combinations was the same in both conditions. Here, an increase in the number of to-be-retrieved S-R rules was associated with a substantial increase in dual-response costs that were also modulated on a trial-by-trial basis when switching between rules. Taken together, the results shed further light on the dependency of cross-modal action control on both action- and rule-related memory retrieval processes. KW - task rules KW - cross-modal action KW - dual tasks KW - dual-response costs KW - oculomotor control Y1 - 2017 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-157794 VL - 8 IS - 529 ER - TY - THES A1 - Beinicke, Andrea T1 - Career Construction Across the Life Span: Career Choice and Career Development T1 - Karrierekonstruktion über die Lebensspanne: Karriereentscheidung und Karriereentwicklung N2 - This dissertation contributes to deepen our understanding of constructs that play a key role in individuals’ vocational career construction. In this regard, many previous studies have focused exclusively on a specific phase of an individual’s career. Yet, modern societies require continuous investments in one’s career to adapt to changing Environments throughout the life span. Consequently, this dissertation takes a broad approach to capture a wide spectrum of career construction processes. According to Super’s (1990) developmental stage framework, individuals have to manage vocational developmental tasks corresponding to each of the developmental life stages in order to be career mature across the life span. As the two stages exploration and maintenance set the stage for individuals’ future career pathways, they are especially important in individuals’ vocational career construction. Therefore, both of them are addressed in this dissertation. By answering open research questions relevant to career choice in early career stages and to career development in later career stages, this dissertation contributes to the overarching goal of shedding more light on constructs relevant to individuals’ vocational career construction processes across the life span. Beyond the results presented within each study’s horizon, this dissertation aimed at offering practical guidance to career counselors, trainees, and training and development (T&D) professionals. Career counselors and T&D professionals are involved in guiding vocational career construction processes of individuals across the life span. Thus, on the one hand, this dissertation supports career counselors’ work so that they can help deliberating individuals make optimal and effective career choices. On the other hand, this dissertation facilitates T&D professionals’ work so that they can effectively design and evaluate e‐learning and classroom trainings in corporate educational settings. Identifying individuals’ vocational interests combined with cognitive abilities through adequate test measures and maximizing success of learning and success of transfer through fostering evidence‐based transfer support actions will help individuals adapt quickly to the changing nature of work environments in the 21st century and to continue to successfully construct careers across the life span. N2 - Diese Promotion trägt dazu bei, unser Verständnis jener Konstrukte zu vertiefen, die eine entscheidende Rolle bei der beruflichen Karriereplanung von Individuen spielen. Viele bisherige Studien haben sich in dieser Hinsicht ausschließlich auf eine spezielle Karrierephase konzentriert. Jedoch verlangt die moderne Gesellschaft, dass Individuen kontinuierlich in ihre Karriere investieren, um sich über die Lebensspanne hinweg an die ständig verändernden Umweltbedingungen anpassen zu können. Demzufolge wählt diese Promotion einen umfassenden Ansatz mit dem Ziel, ein breiteres Spektrum an Prozessen der Karriereplanung zu erfassen. Laut des Entwicklungsstufenkonzepts über die Lebensspanne von Super (1990) müssen Individuen in jeder der Entwicklungsstufen berufliche Entwicklungsaufgaben meistern, um der Karriere ‐ über die Lebensspanne ‐ gewachsen zu sein. Da die beiden Stufen Exploration und Erhaltung zukünftige Karrierewege bahnen, sind diese bei der beruflichen Karriereplanung von Individuen besonders wichtig und stehen deshalb im Fokus dieser Promotion. Durch die Beantwortung offener Forschungsfragen, die sowohl für die Berufswahl in frühen Karrierestufen als auch für die Karriereentwicklung in späteren Karrierestufen relevant sind, trägt diese Promotion zu dem übergeordneten Ziel bei, Konstrukte zu beleuchten, die für den Prozess der individuellen Karriereplanung über die Lebensspanne bedeutsam sind. Neben den Ergebnissen, die im Rahmen der Studien präsentiert wurden, beabsichtigt diese Promotion Karriereberatern, Lernenden und Personalentwicklern praktische Hilfestellungen zu geben. Karriereberater und Personalentwickler sind an der Lenkung von Prozessen der beruflichen Karriereplanung über die Lebensspanne beteiligt. Deshalb versucht diese Promotion einerseits die Arbeit von Karriereberatern zu unterstützen, damit sie unentschlossenen Individuen dabei helfen können, optimale und effektive Karriereentscheidungen zu treffen. Andererseits versucht diese Promotion Personalentwicklern Unterstützung zu bieten, damit sie Umgebungen für E‐Learning und Präsenztrainings im Unternehmen effektiv gestalten und bewerten können. Die Identifikation individueller beruflicher Interessen zusammen mit kognitiven Fähigkeiten mittels adäquater Testverfahren und die Maximierung des Lern‐ und Transfererfolgs mittels Implementierung evidenzbasierter transferförderlicher Maßnahmen sollen dabei helfen, sich schnell an die Veränderungen der Arbeitsumgebungen des 21. Jahrhunderts anzupassen, und eine erfolgreiche Karriere über die Lebensspanne zu durchlaufen. KW - Karriere KW - Beruf KW - career construction KW - vocational interests KW - training evaluation KW - Erwachsener KW - Psychologie KW - career choice KW - career development Y1 - 2017 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-117447 ER - TY - THES A1 - Wirth, Robert T1 - Consequences of bending and breaking the rules T1 - Konsequenzen von Regelbrüchen N2 - Social life is organized around rules and norms. The present experiments investigate the cognitive architecture of rule violations. To do so, a setting with arbitrary rules that had to be followed or broken was developed, and breaking these rules did not have any negative consequences. Removed from any social influences that might further encourage or hinder the rule breaker, results suggest that simply labeling a behavior as a rule violation comes with specific costs: They are more difficult to plan and come with specific behavioral markers during execution. In essence, rule violations resemble rule negations, but they also trigger additional processes. The question of what makes rule violations more difficult than rule inversions is the major focus of the remaining experiments. These experiments revealed negative affective consequences of rule violation and rule inversions alike, while rule violations additionally prime authority-related concepts, thus sensitizing towards authority related stimuli. Next, the question how these burdens of non-conformity can be mitigated was investigated, and the influence of having executed the behavior in question frequently and recently was tested in both negations and rule violations. The burdens of non-conformity can best be reduced by a combination of having violated/negated a rule very frequently and very recently. Transfer from another task, however, could not be identified. To conclude, a model that accounts for the data that is currently presented is proposed. As a variant of a task switching model, it describes the cognitive processes that were investigated and highlights unique processing steps that rule violations seem to require. N2 - Soziales Miteinander ist durch Regeln und Normen organisiert. Die hier beschriebenen Experimente untersuchen die kognitive Architektur von absichtsvollen Regelverstößen. Dazu wurde ein Setting entwickelt, in dem Regeln befolgt oder gebrochen werden mussten, und das Brechen dieser Regeln keine negativen Konsequenzen nach sich zog. Selbst ohne soziale Unterstützung, die das Brechen von Regeln leichter oder schwerer machen könnte, fanden wir, dass allein das Bezeichnen eines Verhaltens als Regelverletzung spezifische Kosten erzeugte: Die Planung dieses Verhaltens ist deutlich erschwert, und die Ausführung zeigt spezifische Verhaltensmuster. Regelverletzungen ähneln hierbei im weitesten Sinne Negationen, aber beinhalten zusätzliche Komponenten. Die Frage wie genau sich die kognitive Kontrolle regelwidriger Verhaltensweisen von der Verarbeitung von Negationen unterscheidet, steht im Zentrum der vorliegenden Arbeit. Die folgenden Experimente zeigen darüber hinaus neben negativen affektiven Konsequenzen, die sowohl Regelbrüche als auch Negationen vorweisen, insbesondere eine direkte Bahnung autoritätsbezogener Konzepte, die eine spezifische Begleiterscheinung absichtsvoller Regelverstöße darstellt. Als nächstes wurde getestet, wie die kognitiven Kosten von Regelverletzungen durch kürzliche oder häufige Ausführung gemindert werden können. Hier zeigte sich, dass die Kombination aus beiden Faktoren die größte Reduktion kognitiver Kosten des Regelverstoßes erbrachte. Ein Transfer von kognitiver Kontrolle von einer anderen Aufgabe konnte jedoch nicht beobachtet werden. Ein Modell, das die hier dargestellten empirischen Ergebnisse vereint, wird abschließend diskutiert. Als Variation eines Modells zum Aufgabenwechsel erklärt es die kognitiven Prozesse, die einer Regelverletzung unterliegen und zeigt Verarbeitungsschritte auf, die für Regelverletzungen spezifisch sind KW - rule violations KW - Soziale Norm KW - Regelverstoß Y1 - 2017 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-155075 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Erlbeck, Helena A1 - Mochty, Ursula A1 - Kübler, Andrea A1 - Real, Ruben G. L. T1 - Circadian course of the P300 ERP in patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis - implications for brain-computer interfaces (BCI) JF - BMC Neurology N2 - Background: Accidents or neurodegenerative diseases like amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) can lead to progressing, extensive, and complete paralysis leaving patients aware but unable to communicate (locked-in state). Brain-computer interfaces (BCI) based on electroencephalography represent an important approach to establish communication with these patients. The most common BCI for communication rely on the P300, a positive deflection arising in response to rare events. To foster broader application of BCIs for restoring lost function, also for end-users with impaired vision, we explored whether there were specific time windows during the day in which a P300 driven BCI should be preferably applied. Methods: The present study investigated the influence of time of the day and modality (visual vs. auditory) on P300 amplitude and latency. A sample of 14 patients (end-users) with ALS and 14 healthy age matched volunteers participated in the study and P300 event-related potentials (ERP) were recorded at four different times (10, 12 am, 2, & 4 pm) during the day. Results: Results indicated no differences in P300 amplitudes or latencies between groups (ALS patients v. healthy participants) or time of measurement. In the auditory condition, latencies were shorter and amplitudes smaller as compared to the visual condition. Conclusion: Our findings suggest applicability of EEG/BCI sessions in patients with ALS throughout normal waking hours. Future studies using actual BCI systems are needed to generalize these findings with regard to BCI effectiveness/efficiency and other times of day. KW - brain computer interface KW - amyotrophic lateral sclerosis KW - ALS KW - P300 KW - auditory KW - visual KW - BCI Y1 - 2017 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-157423 VL - 17 IS - 3 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Käthner, Ivo A1 - Halder, Sebastian A1 - Hintermüller, Christoph A1 - Espinosa, Arnau A1 - Guger, Christoph A1 - Miralles, Felip A1 - Vargiu, Eloisa A1 - Dauwalder, Stefan A1 - Rafael-Palou, Xavier A1 - Solà, Marc A1 - Daly, Jean M. A1 - Armstrong, Elaine A1 - Martin, Suzanne A1 - Kübler, Andrea T1 - A Multifunctional Brain-Computer Interface Intended for Home Use: An Evaluation with Healthy Participants and Potential End Users with Dry and Gel-Based Electrodes JF - Frontiers in Neuroscience N2 - Current brain-computer interface (BCIs) software is often tailored to the needs of scientists and technicians and therefore complex to allow for versatile use. To facilitate home use of BCIs a multifunctional P300 BCI with a graphical user interface intended for non-expert set-up and control was designed and implemented. The system includes applications for spelling, web access, entertainment, artistic expression and environmental control. In addition to new software, it also includes new hardware for the recording of electroencephalogram (EEG) signals. The EEG system consists of a small and wireless amplifier attached to a cap that can be equipped with gel-based or dry contact electrodes. The system was systematically evaluated with a healthy sample, and targeted end users of BCI technology, i.e., people with a varying degree of motor impairment tested the BCI in a series of individual case studies. Usability was assessed in terms of effectiveness, efficiency and satisfaction. Feedback of users was gathered with structured questionnaires. Two groups of healthy participants completed an experimental protocol with the gel-based and the dry contact electrodes (N = 10 each). The results demonstrated that all healthy participants gained control over the system and achieved satisfactory to high accuracies with both gel-based and dry electrodes (average error rates of 6 and 13%). Average satisfaction ratings were high, but certain aspects of the system such as the wearing comfort of the dry electrodes and design of the cap, and speed (in both groups) were criticized by some participants. Six potential end users tested the system during supervised sessions. The achieved accuracies varied greatly from no control to high control with accuracies comparable to that of healthy volunteers. Satisfaction ratings of the two end-users that gained control of the system were lower as compared to healthy participants. The advantages and disadvantages of the BCI and its applications are discussed and suggestions are presented for improvements to pave the way for user friendly BCIs intended to be used as assistive technology by persons with severe paralysis. KW - end-user evaluation KW - brain-computer interface KW - EEG KW - practical electrodes KW - assistive technology Y1 - 2017 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-157925 VL - 11 IS - 286 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Schneider, Wolfgang A1 - Niklas, Frank T1 - Intelligence and verbal short-term memory/working memory: their interrelationships from childhood to young adulthood and their impact on academic achievement JF - Journal of Intelligence N2 - Although recent developmental studies exploring the predictive power of intelligence and working memory (WM) for educational achievement in children have provided evidence for the importance of both variables, findings concerning the relative impact of IQ and WM on achievement have been inconsistent. Whereas IQ has been identified as the major predictor variable in a few studies, results from several other developmental investigations suggest that WM may be the stronger predictor of academic achievement. In the present study, data from the Munich Longitudinal Study on the Genesis of Individual Competencies (LOGIC) were used to explore this issue further. The secondary data analysis included data from about 200 participants whose IQ and WM was first assessed at the age of six and repeatedly measured until the ages of 18 and 23. Measures of reading, spelling, and math were also repeatedly assessed for this age range. Both regression analyses based on observed variables and latent variable structural equation modeling (SEM) were carried out to explore whether the predictive power of IQ and WM would differ as a function of time point of measurement (i.e., early vs. late assessment). As a main result of various regression analyses, IQ and WM turned out to be reliable predictors of academic achievement, both in early and later developmental stages, when previous domain knowledge was not included as additional predictor. The latter variable accounted for most of the variance in more comprehensive regression models, reducing the impact of both IQ and WM considerably. Findings from SEM analyses basically confirmed this outcome, indicating IQ impacts on educational achievement in the early phase, and illustrating the strong additional impact of previous domain knowledge on achievement at later stages of development. KW - intelligence KW - short-term memory KW - working memory KW - academic achievement KW - domain knowledge KW - LOGIC study Y1 - 2017 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-198004 SN - 2079-3200 VL - 5 IS - 2 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Steinborn, Michael B. A1 - Huestegge, Lynn T1 - Phone conversation while processing information: chronometric analysis of load effects in everyday-media multitasking JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - This is a pilot study that examined the effect of cell-phone conversation on cognition using a continuous multitasking paradigm. Current theorizing argues that phone conversation affects behavior (e.g., driving) by interfering at a level of cognitive processes (not peripheral activity) and by implying an attentional-failure account. Within the framework of an intermittent spare–utilized capacity threading model, we examined the effect of aspects of (secondary-task) phone conversation on (primary-task) continuous arithmetic performance, asking whether phone use makes components of automatic and controlled information-processing (i.e., easy vs. hard mental arithmetic) run more slowly, or alternatively, makes processing run less reliably albeit with the same processing speed. The results can be summarized as follows: While neither expecting a text message nor expecting an impending phone call had any detrimental effects on performance, active phone conversation was clearly detrimental to primary-task performance. Crucially, the decrement imposed by secondary-task (conversation) was not due to a constant slowdown but is better be characterized by an occasional breakdown of information processing, which differentially affected automatic and controlled components of primary-task processing. In conclusion, these findings support the notion that phone conversation makes individuals not constantly slower but more vulnerable to commit attention failure, and in this way, hampers stability of (primary-task) information processing. KW - effort KW - vigilance KW - sustained attention KW - cell phone conversation KW - variability Y1 - 2017 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-158253 VL - 8 IS - 896 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Rösler, Lara A1 - End, Albert A1 - Gamer, Matthias T1 - Orienting towards social features in naturalistic scenes is reflexive JF - PLoS ONE N2 - Saliency-based models of visual attention postulate that, when a scene is freely viewed, attention is predominantly allocated to those elements that stand out in terms of their physical properties. However, eye-tracking studies have shown that saliency models fail to predict gaze behavior accurately when social information is included in an image. Notably, gaze pattern analyses revealed that depictions of human beings are heavily prioritized independent of their low-level physical saliency. What remains unknown, however, is whether the prioritization of such social features is a reflexive or a voluntary process. To investigate the early stages of social attention in more detail, participants viewed photographs of naturalistic scenes with and without social features (i.e., human heads or bodies) for 200 ms while their eye movements were being recorded. We observed significantly more first eye movements to regions containing social features than would be expected from a chance level distribution of saccades. Additionally, a generalized linear mixed model analysis revealed that the social content of a region better predicted first saccade direction than its saliency suggesting that social features partially override the impact of low-level physical saliency on gaze patterns. Given the brief image presentation time that precluded visual exploration, our results provide compelling evidence for a reflexive component in social attention. Moreover, the present study emphasizes the importance of considering social influences for a more coherent understanding of human attentional selection. KW - social features KW - visual attention KW - gaze patterns KW - reflexive component Y1 - 2017 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-170586 VL - 12 IS - 7 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Zürn, Michael A1 - Strack, Fritz T1 - When More Is Better – Consumption Priming Decreases Responders’ Rejections in the Ultimatum Game JF - Frontiers in Psychology N2 - During the past decades, economic theories of rational choice have been exposed to outcomes that were severe challenges to their claim of universal validity. For example, traditional theories cannot account for refusals to cooperate if cooperation would result in higher payoffs. A prominent illustration are responders’ rejections of positive but unequal payoffs in the Ultimatum Game. To accommodate this anomaly in a rational framework one needs to assume both a preference for higher payoffs and a preference for equal payoffs. The current set of studies shows that the relative weight of these preference components depends on external conditions and that consumption priming may decrease responders’ rejections of unequal payoffs. Specifically, we demonstrate that increasing the accessibility of consumption-related information accentuates the preference for higher payoffs. Furthermore, consumption priming increased responders’ reaction times for unequal payoffs which suggests an increased conflict between both preference components. While these results may also be integrated into existing social preference models, we try to identify some basic psychological processes underlying economic decision making. Going beyond the Ultimatum Game, we propose that a distinction between comparative and deductive evaluations may provide a more general framework to account for various anomalies in behavioral economics. KW - Ultimatum Game KW - comparison KW - consumption priming KW - evaluation KW - cognitive processes Y1 - 2017 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-189989 SN - 1664-1078 VL - 8 IS - 2226 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Wallmann-Sperlich, Birgit A1 - Bipp, Tanja A1 - Bucksch, Jens A1 - Froboese, Ingo T1 - Who uses height-adjustable desks? - Sociodemographic, health-related, and psycho-social variables of regular users JF - International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity N2 - Background: Sit-to-stand height-adjustable desks (HAD) may promote workplace standing, as long as workers use them on a regular basis. The aim of this study was to investigate (i) how common HAD in German desk-based workers are, and how frequently HADs are used, (ii) to identify sociodemographic, health-related, and psycho-social variables of workday sitting including having a HAD, and (iii) to analyse sociodemographic, health-related, and psycho-social variables of users and non-users of HADs. Methods: A cross-sectional sample of 680 participants (51.9% men; 41.0 ± 13.1 years) in a desk-based occupation was interviewed by telephone about their occupational sitting and standing proportions, having and usage of a HAD, and answered questions concerning psycho-social variables of occupational sitting. The proportion of workday sitting was calculated for participants having an HAD (n = 108) and not-having an HAD (n = 573), as well as for regular users of HAD (n = 54), and irregular/non-users of HAD (n = 54). Linear regressions were conducted to calculate associations between socio-demographic, health-related, psychosocial variables and having/not having an HAD, and the proportion of workday sitting. Logistic regressions were executed to examine the association of mentioned variables and participants’ usage of HADs. Results: Sixteen percent report that they have an HAD, and 50% of these report regular use of HAD. Having an HAD is not a correlate of the proportion of workday sitting. Further analysis restricted to participants having available a HAD highlights that only the ‘perceived advantages of sitting less’ was significantly associated with HAD use in the fully adjusted model (OR 1.75 [1.09; 2.81], p < 0.05). Conclusions: The present findings indicate that accompanying behavioral action while providing an HAD is promising to increase the regular usage of HAD. Hence, future research needs to address the specificity of behavioral actions in order to enhance regular HAD use, and needs to give more fundamental insights into these associations. KW - cross-sectional KW - office-workers KW - desk-based KW - height-adjustable desk KW - occupational sitting and physical activity questionnaire KW - sitting time KW - correlates KW - natural approach Y1 - 2017 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-157888 VL - 14 IS - 26 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Shiban, Youssef A1 - Diemer, Julia A1 - Müller, Jana A1 - Brütting-Schick, Johanna A1 - Pauli, Paul A1 - Mühlberger, Andreas T1 - Diaphragmatic breathing during virtual reality exposure therapy for aviophobia: functional coping strategy or avoidance behavior? A pilot study JF - BMC Psychiatry N2 - Background: Although there is solid evidence for the efficacy of in vivo and virtual reality (VR) exposure therapy for a specific phobia, there is a significant debate over whether techniques promoting distraction or relaxation have impairing or enhancing effects on treatment outcome. In the present pilot study, we investigated the effect of diaphragmatic breathing (DB) as a relaxation technique during VR exposure treatment. Method: Twenty-nine patients with aviophobia were randomly assigned to VR exposure treatment either with or without diaphragmatic breathing (six cycles per minute). Subjective fear ratings, heart rate and skin conductance were assessed as indicators of fear during both the exposure and the test session one week later. Results: The group that experienced VR exposure combined with diaphragmatic breathing showed a higher tendency to effectively overcome the fear of flying. Psychophysiological measures of fear decreased and self-efficacy increased in both groups with no significant difference between the groups. Conclusions: Our findings indicate that diaphragmatic breathing during VR exposure does not interfere with the treatment outcome and may even enhance treatment effects of VR exposure therapy for aviophobic patients. KW - Virtual reality KW - Exposure therapy KW - Diaphragmatic breathing KW - Aviophobia KW - Experimental study Y1 - 2017 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-181007 VL - 17 ER -